Autism: Difference between revisions

From Citizendium
Jump to navigation Jump to search
imported>Thomas Simmons
(→‎DSM definition: reformated title category)
mNo edit summary
 
(82 intermediate revisions by 18 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Infobox_Disease
{{subpages}}
  | Name = Autism
{{Infobox Disease |
| Image =
  | Name       = Autism |
| Caption =
  | ICD10     = [http://www.who.int/classifications/apps/icd/icd10online/?gf80.htm+f840 F84.0-F84.1] |
  | ICD10 = {{ICD10|F|84|0|f|80}}
  | ICD9       = [http://www.icd9data.com/2007/Volume1/290-319/295-299/299/299.0.htm ICD9 299] |
  | ICD9 = {{ICD9|299.0}}
  | OMIM     = 209850 |
| ICDO =
  | MedlinePlus   = 001526 |
  | OMIM = 209850
  | MedlinePlus = 001526
| eMedicineSubj = med
| eMedicineTopic = 3202
| eMedicine_mult = {{eMedicine2|ped|180}}
| MeshID =
}}
}}


'''Autism''' is classified by the [[World Health Organization]] and [[American Psychological Association]] as a developmental disability that results from a disorder of the human [[central nervous system]]. It is diagnosed using specific criteria for impairments to social interaction, communication, interests, imagination and activities.<ref>[http://www.iidc.indiana.edu/irca/generalinfo/diagnost.html Diagnostic Criteria for Autistic Disorder] Indiana Resource Center for Autism, Indiana Institute on Disability and Community, Indiana University. Retrieved Feb. 27, 2007</ref> The causes, symptoms, etiology, treatment, and other issues are [[controversial]].
'''Autism''' (pronounced [[IPA]] /'ɔtizm/) is classified by the [[World Health Organization]] and [[American Psychological Association]] as a developmental disability that results from a disorder of the human [[central nervous system]]. It is diagnosed using specific criteria for impairments to social interaction, communication, interests, imagination and activities.<ref>[http://www.iidc.indiana.edu/irca/generalinfo/diagnost.html Diagnostic Criteria for Autistic Disorder] Indiana Resource Center for Autism, Indiana Institute on Disability and Community, Indiana University. Retrieved Feb. 27, 2007</ref>


Autism manifests itself "before the age of three years" according to the World Health Organization's International Classification of Diseases ([[ICD|ICD-10]])<ref>[http://www.who.int/classifications/apps/icd/icd10online/] ICD version 2006, World Health Organisation Codes for Pervasive developmental disorders, F84.0-F84.9, retrieved 20 January, 2007</ref> Autistic children are marked by delays in their "social interaction, language as used in social communication, or symbolic or imaginative play" (''[[Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders]]'').<ref>more commonly known as the DSM-IV-TR, i.e. the fourth edition with text revision. Relevant DSM codes are: 307.9 Communication Disorder Not Otherwise Specified, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 299.00 Autistic Disorder, 299.80 Rett's Disorder, 299.10 Childhood Disintegrative Disorder, 299.80 Asperger's Disorder, 299.80 Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (Including Atypical Autism), Attention-Deficit and Disruptive Behavior Disorders, Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder</ref>
Autism manifests before the age of three years, according to the World Health Organization's International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10)<ref>[http://www.who.int/classifications/apps/icd/icd10online/] ICD version 2006, World Health Organisation Codes for Pervasive developmental disorders, F84.0-F84.9, retrieved 20 January, 2007</ref>. Autistic children are marked by delays in their "social interaction, language as used in social communication, or symbolic or imaginative play" (''Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders'').<ref>more commonly known as the DSM-IV-TR, i.e. the fourth edition with text revision. Relevant DSM codes are: 307.9 Communication Disorder Not Otherwise Specified, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 299.00 Autistic Disorder, 299.80 Rett's Disorder, 299.10 Childhood Disintegrative Disorder, 299.80 Asperger's Disorder, 299.80 Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (Including Atypical Autism), Attention-Deficit and Disruptive Behavior Disorders, Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder</ref>


Autism, and the other four [[pervasive developmental disorder]]s (PDD), are all considered to be [[neurodevelopmental disorders]]. They are diagnosed on the basis of a triad, or group of three behavioral impairments or dysfunctions: 1. impaired social interaction, 2. impaired communication and 3. restricted and repetitive interests and activities.<ref> American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 1994)</ref> These three basic characteristics reflect Dr. [[Leo Kanner]]'s first reports of autism emphasizing "autistic aloneness" and "insistence on sameness."
Autism and the other four [[pervasive developmental disorder]]s (PDD) are considered to be [[neurodevelopmental disorder]]s. They are diagnosed on the basis of a triad of behavioral impairments or dysfunctions: 1. impaired social interaction, 2. impaired communication and 3. restricted and repetitive interests and activities.<ref> American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 1994)</ref>  


From a physiological standpoint, autism is often less than obvious in that outward appearance may not indicate a disorder. Diagnosis typically comes from a complete patient history and physical and [[Neurology|neurological]] evaluation.
From a physiological standpoint, autism is often less than obvious, in that outward appearance may not indicate a disorder. Diagnosis typically comes from a complete patient history and physical and [[Neurology|neurological]] evaluation.


The incidence of diagnosed autism has increased since the 1990s.<ref>[http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/5174144.stm "Autism 'more common than thought'"] BBC News (2006) July 13</ref> Reasons offered for this phenomenon include better diagnosis, wider public awareness of the condition, regional variations in diagnostic criteria, or simply an increase in the occurrence of ASD ([[autism spectrum disorders]]). The United States [[Centers for Disease Control and Prevention|Centers for Disease Control]] (CDC) estimate the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders to be about one in every 150 children.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.abcnews.go.com/Health/wireStory?id=2859985 | title=ABC News Autism More Common Than Thought in U.S.: Survey>|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref><ref>[http://www.forbes.com/feeds/ap/2007/02/08/ap3410772.html U.S. Says Autism Rate 1 in 150] Mike Stobbe. Forbes.com 2 Feb. 2007. Retrieved Feb 9, 2007</ref> In 2005, the [[National Institute of Mental Health]] (NIMH) stated the "best conservative estimate" as 1 in 1000.<ref>{{cite web | title=NIH Autism Overview 2005 | url=http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/upload/autism_overview_2005.pdf | accessdate = 2006-02-05}}</ref> In 2006, NIMH estimated that the incidence was 2-6 in every 1000<ref>[http://www.nimh.nih.gov/press/autism.irp.trials.cfm New NIMH Research Program Launches Autism Trials (page 4)] National Institute of Mental Health. September 7, 2006</ref>
The incidence of diagnosed autism has increased since the 1990s.<ref>[http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/5174144.stm "Autism 'more common than thought'"] BBC News (2006) July 13</ref> Reasons offered for this phenomenon include better diagnosis, wider public awareness of the condition, regional variations in diagnostic criteria, or an actual increase in the occurrence of ASD ([[autism spectrum disorders]]). The United States Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) estimate the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders to be about one in every 150 children.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.abcnews.go.com/Health/wireStory?id=2859985 | title=ABC News Autism More Common Than Thought in U.S.: Survey>|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref><ref>[http://www.forbes.com/feeds/ap/2007/02/08/ap3410772.html U.S. Says Autism Rate 1 in 150] Mike Stobbe. Forbes.com 2 Feb. 2007. Retrieved Feb 9, 2007</ref> In 2005, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) stated the "best conservative estimate" as 1 in 1000.<ref>{{cite web | title=NIH Autism Overview 2005 | url=http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/upload/autism_overview_2005.pdf | accessdate = 2006-02-05}}</ref> In 2006, NIMH estimated that the incidence was 2-6 in every 1000<ref>[http://www.nimh.nih.gov/press/autism.irp.trials.cfm New NIMH Research Program Launches Autism Trials (page 4)] National Institute of Mental Health. September 7, 2006</ref>


There are numerous theories as to the specific [[causes of autism]], but they are as yet unproven (see section on "Causes" below). Proposed factors include genetic influence, anatomical variations (e.g. head circumference), abnormal blood vessel function and [[oxidative stress]]. Their significance as well as implications for treatment remain speculative.
There are numerous theories as to the specific causes of autism, but they are as yet unproven (see section on "Causes" below). Proposed factors include genetic influence, anatomical variations (e.g. head circumference), abnormal blood vessel function and oxidative stress. Their significance as well as implications for treatment remain speculative.


Conversely, some autistic children and adults are opposed to attempts to cure autism. These people see autism as part of who they are,<ref>[[Jim Sinclair|Sinclair, Jim]]. "Do not Mourn for Us." http://ani.autistics.org/dont_mourn.html Accessed: 23 Jan. 2007</ref><ref>The People at Autistics.Org http://www.autistics.org/library/dawson.html Accessed: 23 Jan 2007</ref><ref>[[Amy Harmon|Harmon, Amy]]. "How About not Curing Us? Some Autistics are Pleading." [[New York Times]]. [[20 December]] [[2004]]. A copy on the web was accessed at the URL http://www.co-brass.com/articles_how_about_not_curing_us.htm on 23 January 2007</ref> and in some cases they perceive treatments and attempts of a cure to be unethical.<ref>[[Michelle Dawson|Dawson, Michelle]]. "The Misbehavior of Behavioralists." [[18 January]] [[2004]]. http://www.sentex.net/~nexus23/naa_aba.html. Accessed: [[23 January]] [[2007]].</ref>
==Terminology==
When referring to someone who is diagnosed with autism, the term "autistic" is often used. Alternatively, many prefer to use the person-first terminology, i.e. "person with autism" or "person who experiences autism." However, it has been noted that members of the autistic community generally prefer "autistic person" for reasons that are fairly controversial.<ref> [http://www.autismsocietyofwa.org/DiaryJuly-Aug05.pdf The Diary, July-August 2005], publication of the Autism Society of Washington, page 4, accessed 4 February 2007  Autistic adults at the Autism Society of America 2005 Conference felt that the term "individuals with autism" separates their autism from who they are. In other words, they believe their autism is part of who they are and want to be called "autistic adults.</ref> This article uses both terminologies.


==History==
The word "autism" was first used in the English language by Swiss psychiatrist Eugene Bleuler in a 1912 issue of the ''American Journal of Insanity''. It comes from the Greek word for "self," αυτος (''autos'').<ref>[http://www.pediatricservices.com/prof/prof-26.htm Autism Through Ages Baffles Science] Robert Williams Jr., Pediatric Services</ref> Autism was actually confused with [[schizophrenia]] during the early stages of observation.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.autism.org/interview/ljk.html|title=Interview with Lorna Jean King, OTR, FAOTA|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref> Bleuler used the term to describe schizophrenics' seeming difficulty in connecting with other people.<ref>[http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?CMD=search&DB=pubmed "Epistemological aspects of Eugen Bleuler's conception of schizophrenia in 1911."] Stotz-Ingenlath G., Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy. 2000;3(2):153-9. ISSN:1386-7423</ref>


However, the medical classification of autism as a separate disorder or disease did not occur until 1943 when psychiatrist Dr. Leo Kanner of the Johns Hopkins Hospital in Baltimore, Maryland reported on 11 child patients with striking behavioral similarities and introduced the label "early infantile autism."<ref>[http://www.nimh.nih.gov/publicat/autism.cfm "Autism Spectrum Disorders (Pervasive Developmental Disorders)"] National Institute of Mental Health</ref> He suggested the term "autism" to describe the fact that the children seemed to lack interest in other people, emphasizing "autistic aloneness" and "insistence on sameness". Kanner's first paper on the subject was published in the journal ''The Nervous Child'' (no longer in publication),<ref>Kanner, L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. ''Nervous Child'' 2, 217–250 (1943)</ref> and almost every characteristic he originally described is still regarded as typical of the autistic spectrum of disorders diagnostic triad of behavioral impairments or dysfunctions: 1. impaired social interaction, 2. impaired communication and 3. restricted and repetitive interests and activities.<ref> American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 1994)</ref>.<ref>[http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v9/n10/full/nn1770.html Time to give up on a single explanation for autism] Francesca Happé, Angelica Ronald & Robert Plomin (2006) Nature Neuroscience - 9, 1218 - 1220.  Published online: 26 September 2006</ref>


== '''History''' ==
Leo Kanner's contemporary in Austria, Dr. Hans Asperger, made similar observations, although his name has since become attached to a different form of autism known as [[Asperger syndrome|Asperger's Syndrome]]. Widespread recognition of Asperger's work was delayed by World War II in Germany. His seminal paper was not actually translated into English for almost 50 years and the majority of his work was not widely read until 1997.<ref>[http://www.udel.edu/bkirby/asperger/aswhatisit.html "What Is Asperger Syndrome?"] Barbara L. Kirby, Online Asperger Syndrome Information and Support</ref>
The word '''autism''' was first used in the [[English language]] by [[Switzerland|Swiss]] psychiatrist [[Eugene Bleuler]] in a 1912 issue of the ''American Journal of Insanity''. It comes from the [[Greek language|Greek]] word for "self," αυτος (''autos'').<ref>[http://www.pediatricservices.com/prof/prof-26.htm Autism Through Ages Baffles Science] Robert Williams Jr., Pediatric Services</ref> Autism was actually confused with [[schizophrenia]] during the early stages of observation.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.autism.org/interview/ljk.html|title=Interview with Lorna Jean King, OTR, FAOTA|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref> Bleuler used the term to describe the schizophrenics' seeming difficulty in connecting with other people.<ref>[http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?CMD=search&DB=pubmed "Epistemological aspects of Eugen Bleuler's conception of schizophrenia in 1911."] Stotz-Ingenlath G., Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy. 2000;3(2):153-9. ISSN:1386-7423</ref>


However, the [[Medical classification|classification]] of autism as a separate disorder or disease did not occur until 1943 when psychiatrist Dr. [[Leo Kanner]] of the [[Johns Hopkins Hospital]] in [[Baltimore, Maryland|Baltimore]] reported on 11 child patients with striking behavioral similarities and introduced the label "early infantile autism."<ref>[http://www.nimh.nih.gov/publicat/autism.cfm "Autism Spectrum Disorders (Pervasive Developmental Disorders)"] National Institute of Mental Health</ref> He suggested the term "autism" to describe the fact that the children seemed to lack interest in other people. Kanner's first paper on the subject was published in a now defunct journal called ''The Nervous Child'',<ref>Kanner, L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. ''Nervous Child'' 2, 217–250 (1943)</ref> and almost every characteristic he originally described is still regarded as typical of the autistic spectrum of disorders.<ref>[http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v9/n10/full/nn1770.html Time to give up on a single explanation for autism] Francesca Happé, Angelica Ronald & Robert Plomin (2006) Nature Neuroscience - 9, 1218 - 1220. Published online: 26 September 2006</ref>
Autism and Asperger's Syndrome are today listed in the [[Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders|DSM-IV-TR]] as two of the five [[pervasive developmental disorder]]s (PDD), which also include [[Childhood disintegrative disorder]],<ref>*{{cite journal | author=Frombonne E. | title=Prevalence of childhood disintegrative disorder | journal=Autism | year=2002 | volume=6 | issue=2 | pages=149-157}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | author=Volkmar RM and Rutter M. | title=Childhood disintegrative disorder: Results of the DSM-IV autism field trial | journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | year=1995 | volume=34 | pages=1092-1095}}</ref> [[Rett syndrome]]<ref>{{cite web | author= | title=Rett syndrome (NIH Publication No. 01-4960) | publisher=Rockville, MD: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development | year=2001 | work=Rett syndrome | url=http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubskey.cfm?from=autism | accessmonthday = July 30 | accessyear= 2005 }}</ref> and [[PDD not otherwise specified|Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified]] (or atypical autism). Health care providers also refer to [[Autistic spectrum|autism spectrum disorders]] (ASD) which includes only three of those listed in PDD: Autistic disorder, Asperger syndrome, Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified.<ref name=NICHD-screen>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/upload/autism_overview_2005.pdf "Autism Overview: What we know"] Filipek et al. (1999). Screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. ''Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(6)'': 439-484. Cited in NICHD publications reference. Retrieved 26 Jan. 2007</ref> All of these conditions are characterized by varying degrees of deficiencies in communication skil]s and social interactions, along with restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of human behavior.


At the same time, an [[Austria]]n scientist named Dr. [[Hans Asperger]] made similar observations, although his name has since become attached to a different higher-functioning form of autism known as [[Asperger syndrome]]. Widespread recognition of Asperger's work was delayed by [[World War II]] in [[Germany]], and by his seminal paper not being translated into English for almost 50 years. The majority of his work was not widely read until 1997.<ref>[http://www.udel.edu/bkirby/asperger/aswhatisit.html "What Is Asperger Syndrome?"] Barbara L. Kirby, Online Asperger Syndrome Information and Support</ref>
==Characteristics==
On the surface, individuals who have autism are physically indistinguishable from those without. Some studies show that autistic children tend to have larger head circumferences<ref>[http://www.courchesneautismlab.org/headcircumference.html Center for Autism Research article on head circumference] Retrieved November 13, 2006</ref><ref>Lainhart JE, Bigler ED, Bocian M, Coon H, Dinh E, Dawson G, Deutsch CK, Dunn M, Estes A, Tager-Flusberg H, Folstein S, Hepburn S, Hyman S, McMahon W, Minshew N, Munson J, Osann K, Ozonoff S, Rodier P, Rogers S, Sigman M, Spence MA, Stodgell CJ, Volkmar F. 2006. >[http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/cgi-bin/abstract/113388509/ABSTRACT on-line abstract]</ref> but the significance in the disorder is unclear. Sometimes autism co-occurs with other disorders, and in those cases outward differences may be apparent.


Autism and Asperger's Syndrome are today listed in the [[Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders|DSM-IV-TR]] as two of the five [[pervasive developmental disorder]]s (PDD), which also include [[Childhood disintegrative disorder]],<ref>*{{cite journal | author=Frombonne E. | title=Prevalence of childhood disintegrative disorder | journal=Autism | year=2002 | volume=6 | issue=2 | pages=149-157}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | author=Volkmar RM and Rutter M. | title=Childhood disintegrative disorder: Results of the DSM-IV autism field trial | journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | year=1995 | volume=34 | pages=1092-1095}}</ref> [[Rett syndrome]]<ref>{{cite web | author= | title=Rett syndrome (NIH Publication No. 01-4960) | publisher=Rockville, MD: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development | year=2001 | work=Rett syndrome | url=http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubskey.cfm?from=autism | accessmonthday = July 30 | accessyear= 2005 }}</ref> and [[PDD not otherwise specified|Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified]] (or atypical autism). Health care providers also refer to [[Autistic spectrum|autism spectrum disorders]] (ASD) which includes only three of those listed in PDD: Autistic disorder, Asperger syndrome, Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified.<ref>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/upload/autism_overview_2005.pdf ''Autism Overview: What We Know''] National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Retrieved 26 January, 2007</ref> All of these conditions are characterized by varying degrees of deficiencies in [[communication skill]]s and social interactions, along with restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of [[Human behavior|behavior]].
Individuals diagnosed with autism can vary greatly in skills and behaviors, and their response to sensory input shows marked differences in a number of ways from that of other people. Certain [[stimulus|stimuli]], such as sounds, lights, and touch, will often affect someone with autism differently than someone without, and the degree to which the sensory system is affected can vary greatly from one individual to another.<ref>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/news/releases/autism_affects_brain.cfm "Study Provides Evidence That Autism Affects Functioning of Entire Brain-Previous View Held Autism Limited to Communication, Social Behavior, and Reasoning"] National Institutes of Health, (Aug, 16, 2006)</ref>  


='''Characteristics'''=
===Key behaviors===
On the surface, individuals who have autism are physically indistinguishable from those without. Some studies show that autistic children tend to have larger head circumferences<ref>[http://www.courchesneautismlab.org/headcircumference.html Center for Autism Research article on head circumference] Retrieved [[November 13]] [[2006]]</ref><ref>Lainhart JE, Bigler ED, Bocian M, Coon H, Dinh E, Dawson G, Deutsch CK, Dunn M, Estes A, Tager-Flusberg H, Folstein S, Hepburn S, Hyman S, McMahon W, Minshew N, Munson J, Osann K, Ozonoff S, Rodier P, Rogers S, Sigman M, Spence MA, Stodgell CJ, Volkmar F. 2006. >[http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/cgi-bin/abstract/113388509/ABSTRACT on-line abstract]</ref> but the significance in the disorder is unclear. Sometimes autism co-occurs with other disorders, and in those cases outward differences may be apparent.
Autistic children may display unusual behaviors or fail to display expected behaviors. Normal behaviors may develop at the appropriate age and then disappear or, conversely, are delayed and develop quite some time after normal occurrence. In assessing developmental delays, different physicians may not always arrive at the same conclusions. Much of this difference between diagnosis is due to the disputed criteria for autism.<ref>[http://www.maapservices.org/MAAP_Sub_Find_It_-_Publications_Ehlers_and_Gillberg_Article.htm The Epidemiology of Asperger Syndrome: A Total Population Study] Stephan Ehlers and Christopher Gillberg (1993) The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Vol. 34, No. 8, pp. 1327-1350, November. Reprinted with Permission by Cambridge University Press. This seminal article discusses the nature of variations in diagnostic criteria in Asperger's and Autism</ref> Disagreement on deciding how a child should normally behave also makes it difficult to construct objective tests of child behavior.  


Individuals diagnosed with autism can vary greatly in skills and behaviors, and their response to sensory input shows marked differences in a number of ways from that of other people. Certain [[stimulation]]s, such as sounds, lights, and touch, will often affect someone with autism differently than someone without, and the degree to which the sensory system is affected can vary greatly from one individual to another.<ref>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/news/releases/autism_affects_brain.cfm "Study Provides Evidence That Autism Affects Functioning of Entire Brain-Previous View Held Autism Limited to Communication, Social Behavior, and Reasoning"] National Institutes of Health, (Aug, 16, 2006)</ref>
Essentially, the diagnosis of autism must meet specific criteria but there are also many characteristics that are idiosyncratic. Thus, autism is not a "one size fits all" label. As a result, the [[Autistic spectrum|spectrum disorder]] encompasses a very wide range of behaviors and symptoms.


=='''Key behaviors'''==
Some behaviors cited by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (listed below) may simply mean a normal delay in one or more areas of development, while others are more typical of ASDs—Autistic Spectrum Disorders.<ref name=NICHD-screen/>
Autistic children may display unusual behaviors or fail to display expected behaviors. Normal behaviors may develop at the appropriate age and then disappear or, conversely, are delayed and develop quite some time after normal occurrence. In assessing developmental delays, different physicians may not always arrive at the same conclusions. Much of this difference between diagnosis is due to the disputed criteria for autism.<ref>[http://www.maapservices.org/MAAP_Sub_Find_It_-_Publications_Ehlers_and_Gillberg_Article.htm The Epidemiology of Asperger Syndrome: A Total Population Study] Stephan Ehlers and Christopher Gillberg (1993) The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Vol. 34, No. 8, pp. 1327-1350, November. Reprinted with Permission by Cambridge University Press. This seminal article discusses the nature of variations in diagnostic criteria in Asperger's and Autism</ref> Deciding how a child should behave is also difficult because diagnostic tests have to be objective, which is not a simple thing to accomplish.  Because of this practitioners and researchers in [[pediatrics]], [[child psychology]], [[behavior analysis]], and [[child development]] are always looking for early indicators of autism.


The diagnosis of autism must meet specific criterion but there are also many characteristics that are idiosyncratic. Thus, Autism is not a "one size fits all" label. In other words the [[Autistic spectrum|spectrum disorder]] encompasses a very wide range of behaviors and symptoms.
The list below is not all-inclusive, and generally applies to children and not adults. Furthermore, while some of these behaviors might be seen in a person with autism, others may be absent.<ref name=NICHD-screen/>


Some behaviors cited by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (listed below) may simply mean a normal delay in one or more areas of development, while others are more typical of ASDs—Autistic Spectrum Disorders.<ref>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/upload/autism_overview_2005.pdf "Autism Overview: What we know"] Filipek et al. (1999). Screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. ''Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(6)'': 439-484. Cited in NICHD publications reference. Retrieved 26 Jan. 2007</ref>
====Noted behaviours====


The list below is not all-inclusive, and generally applies to children and not adults.  Furthermore, while some of these behaviors might be seen in a person with autism, others may be absent.<ref>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/upload/autism_overview_2005.pdf "Autism Overview: What we know"] Filipek et al. (1999). Screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. ''Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(6)'': 439-484. Cited in NICHD publications reference. Retrieved 26 Jan. 2007</ref>
*does not respond to his/her name
 
*cannot explain what he/she wants
==='''Noted behaviours'''===
*language skills are slow to develop or speech is delayed
 
*does not follow directions
*does not respond to his/her name.
*will fuss if didn't get what wanted
*cannot explain what he/she wants.
*at times, the child seems to be deaf
*language skills are slow to develop or speech is delayed.
*seems to hear sometimes, but not other times
*doesn't follow directions.
*doesn't point or wave "bye-bye"
*will fuss if didn't get what wanted.
*doesn't understand the concept of pointing; will look at the hand pointing rather than the object being pointed at
*at times, the child seems to be deaf.
*used to say a few words or babble, but now he/she doesn't
*seems to hear sometimes, but not other times.
*throws intense or violent tantrums
*doesn't point or wave "bye-bye."
*has odd movement patterns
*doesn't understand the concept of pointing; will look at the hand pointing rather than the object being pointed at.
*likes to spin around in a circle
*used to say a few words or babble, but now he/she doesn't.
*likes being in a well known place
*throws intense or violent tantrums.
*hands often cover ears
*has odd movement patterns.
*is overly active, uncooperative, or resistant
*likes to spin around in a circle.
*doesn't know how to play with toys
*likes being in a place well known.
*doesn't smile when smiled at
*hands on ears oftenly.
*has poor eye contact
*is overly active, uncooperative, or resistant.
*gets "stuck" doing the same things over and over and can't move on to other things
*doesn't know how to play with toys.
*seems to prefer to play alone
*doesn't smile when smiled at.
*gets things for him/herself only
*has poor eye contact.
*is very independent for his/her age
*gets "stuck" doing the same things over and over and can't move on to other things.
*does things "early" compared to other children
*seems to prefer to play alone.
*gets things for him/herself only.
*is very independent for his/her age.
*does things "early" compared to other children.
*seems to be in his/her "own world."
*seems to be in his/her "own world."
*seems to tune people out.
*seems to tune people out
*is not interested in other children.
*is not interested in other children
*dislikes playing pretend.
*dislikes playing pretend
*walks on his/her toes.
*walks on his/her toes
*shows unusual attachments to toys, objects, or schedules (i.e., always holding a string or having to put socks on before pants).
*shows unusual attachments to toys, objects, or schedules (i.e., always holding a string or having to put socks on before pants)
*spends a lot of time stacking objects, lining things up or putting things in a certain order.
*spends a lot of time stacking objects, lining things up or putting things in a certain order
*unconcerned about - or completely oblivious to - dangers around him/her (i.e., standing in the middle of the street without worrying about getting hit by a car).
*unconcerned about - or completely oblivious to - dangers around him/her (e.g., standing in the middle of the street without worrying about getting hit by a car)


=='''Autism and blindness'''==
===Autism and blindness===
The characteristics of a person with both an Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and a severe visual impairment (VI) may vary from a person with just ASD or just VI.<ref>Gense, Marilyn H., and D. Jay Gense. "Autism Spectrum Disorders in Learners with Blindness/Vision Impairments." RE: View (1994). [[5 May]] [[2006]] <http://focusfamilies.org/focus/docs/blindnessandautism.pdf PDF></ref> Historically, many behaviors of blind children were seen as "autistic-like" but were attributed to their blindness rather than pursuing possibilities of autism.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.tsbvi.edu/Education/vmi/autistic-vs-nonautistic.htm|title=Examples of Symptom Presentation in Blind/Autistic Children in Contrast to Blind/Non-Autistic Children a la DSM-IV Criter|accessdate=2007-01-25}} T.Pawletko & L. Rocissano (2000) Texas School for Blind and Visually Impaired</ref>
The characteristics of a person with both an Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and a severe visual impairment (VI) may vary from a person with just ASD or just VI.<ref>Gense, Marilyn H., and D. Jay Gense. "Autism Spectrum Disorders in Learners with Blindness/Vision Impairments." RE: View (1994). 5 May 2006 <http://focusfamilies.org/focus/docs/blindnessandautism.pdf PDF></ref> Historically, many behaviors of blind children were seen as "autistic-like" but were attributed to their blindness rather than pursuing possibilities of autism.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.tsbvi.edu/Education/vmi/autistic-vs-nonautistic.htm|title=Examples of Symptom Presentation in Blind/Autistic Children in Contrast to Blind/Non-Autistic Children a la DSM-IV Criter|accessdate=2007-01-25}} T.Pawletko & L. Rocissano (2000) Texas School for Blind and Visually Impaired</ref>


Developmental trajectories of children with ASD-VI are often very similar as those followed by children with typical autism, but the child with ASD-VI will have particularly unusual responses to sensory information. The person may be overly sensitive to touch or sound, or be less responsive to pain. Typically, touch, smell, and sound are affected the most dramatically.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/autism/detail_autism.htm|title=Info on autism|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref>
Developmental trajectories of children with ASD-VI are often very similar as those followed by children with typical autism, but the child with ASD-VI will have particularly unusual responses to sensory information. The person may be overly sensitive to touch or sound, or be less responsive to pain. Typically, touch, smell, and sound are affected the most dramatically.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/autism/detail_autism.htm|title=Info on autism|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref>


=='''Repetitive behaviors'''==
===Repetitive behaviors===
Although people with autism usually appear physically normal, unusual repetitive motions, known as self-stimulation or "[[stimming]]," may set them apart. These behaviors might be extreme or subtle. Some children and older individuals spend a lot of time repeatedly flapping their arms or wiggling their toes, others suddenly freeze in position. Some spend hours arranging objects in a certain way rather than engaging in pretend play as a typical child might, and becoming agitated if they are re-arranged or moved. Repetitive behaviors can also extend into the spoken word; [[perseveration]] of a single word or phrase can also become a part of the child's daily routine. Some may repeat words from [[movie]]s and watch certain bits over and over again.<ref name=Crosland>{{cite web|url=http://www.lsi.ku.edu/lsi/internal/seminars/presentations/Crosland_K05-25-01.pdf |title=Prevalence of stereotypy among children diagnosed with autism at a tertiary referral clinic |accessdate=2006-07-01 |last=Crosland |first=K.A. |coauthors=''et al.'' |date=2001-05-25|format=pdf |work=Presented at the Association for Behavioral Analysis annual conference }}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://web1.greatbasin.net/~sprang/stimming.htm |title=Stereotypic (Self-Stimulatory) Behavior (Stimming) |accessdate=2006-07-01 |last=Edelson |first=Stephen M. |date=1995}}</ref> Autistic children may demand consistency in their environment. A slight change in the timing, format or route of a routine or trip can be extremely disturbing to them. {{Fact|date=February 2007}}. Autistics sometimes have persistent, intense preoccupations. For example, the child might be obsessed with learning all about [[computers]], [[television program]]s, [[lighthouse]]s or virtually any other topic.<ref>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/upload/autism_overview_2005.pdf "Autism Overview: What we know"] Filipek et al. (1999). Screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. ''Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(6)'': 439-484. Cited in NICHD publications reference. Retrieved 26 Jan. 2007</ref>
Although people with autism usually appear physically normal, unusual repetitive motions, known as self-stimulation or "stimming," may set them apart. These behaviors might be extreme or subtle. Some children and older individuals spend a lot of time repeatedly flapping their arms or wiggling their toes, others suddenly freeze in position. Some spend hours arranging objects in a certain way rather than engaging in pretend play as a typical child might, and becoming agitated if they are re-arranged or moved. Repetitive behaviors can also extend into the spoken word; perseveration of a single word or phrase can also become a part of the child's daily routine. Some may repeat words from movies and watch certain bits over and over again.<ref name=Crosland>{{cite web|url=http://www.lsi.ku.edu/lsi/internal/seminars/presentations/Crosland_K05-25-01.pdf |title=Prevalence of stereotypy among children diagnosed with autism at a tertiary referral clinic |accessdate=2006-07-01 |last=Crosland |first=K.A. |coauthors=''et al.'' |date=2001-05-25|format=pdf |work=Presented at the Association for Behavioral Analysis annual conference }}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://web1.greatbasin.net/~sprang/stimming.htm |title=Stereotypic (Self-Stimulatory) Behavior (Stimming) |accessdate=2006-07-01 |last=Edelson |first=Stephen M. |date=1995}}</ref> Autistic children may demand consistency in their environment. A slight change in the timing, format or route of a routine or trip can be extremely disturbing to them. Autistics sometimes have persistent, intense preoccupations. For example, the child might be obsessed with learning all about [[computers]], television programs, lighthouses or virtually any other topic.<ref name=NICHD-screen/>
 


='''DSM definition'''=
Autism is defined in section 299.00 of the ''[[Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders]]'' (DSM-IV) as:
#A total of six (or more) items from (1), (2) and (3), with at least two from (1), and one each from (2) and (3):
##qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:
###marked impairment in the use of multiple nonverbal behaviors such as eye-to-eye gaze, facial expression, body postures, and gestures to regulate social interaction
###failure to develop peer relationships appropriate to developmental level
###a lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment, interests, or achievements with other people (e.g., by a lack of showing, bringing, or pointing out objects of interest)
###lack of social or emotional reciprocity
##qualitative impairments in communication as manifested by at least one of the following:
###delay in, or total lack of, the development of spoken language (not accompanied by an attempt to compensate through alternative modes of communication such as gesture or mime)
###in individuals with adequate speech, marked impairment in the ability to initiate or sustain a conversation with others
###stereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic language
###lack of varied, spontaneous make-believe play or social imitative play appropriate to developmental level
##restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities, as manifested by at least one of the following:
###encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest that is abnormal either in intensity or focus
###apparently inflexible adherence to specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals
###stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms (e.g., hand or finger flapping or twisting, or complex whole-body movements)
###persistent preoccupation with parts of objects
#Delays or abnormal functioning in at least one of the following areas, with onset prior to age 3 years:
##social interaction
##language as used in social communication
##symbolic or imaginative play.
#The disturbance is not better accounted for by [[Rett syndrome|Rett's Disorder]] or [[Childhood disintegrative disorder|Childhood Disintegrative Disorder]].


==Types of autism ==
==Types of autism==
Autism presents in a wide degree, from those who are nearly [[dysfunctional]] and apparently [[Developmental Disability|mentally disabled]] to those whose symptoms are mild or remedied enough to appear unexceptional ("normal") to others. Although not used or accepted by professionals or within the literature, autistic individuals are often divided into those with an [[Intelligence Quotient|IQ]]<80 referred to as having "low-functioning autism" (LFA), while those with IQ>80 are referred to as having "high-functioning autism" (HFA).<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.nas.org.uk/nas/jsp/polopoly.jsp?d=1049&a=3337|title=Facts on autism|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref> Low and high functioning are more generally applied to how well an individual can accomplish activities of daily living, rather than to [[IQ]]. The terms low and high functioning are controversial and not all autistics accept these labels.  Additionally, a recent review questioned the validity of IQ testing of autistic people.<ref>"Many times, if the researchers had a child they couldn't test, they just assumed he or she was retarded and assigned a low IQ score."  ''Professor challenges autism assumption.''  The Oregonian, November 25, 2006.[http://www.oregonlive.com/news/oregonian/index.ssf?/base/news/1164421506110210.xml&coll=7].  Retrieved 2007-02-25.</ref>
Autism presents in a wide degree, from those who are socially dysfunctional and apparently mentally disabled to those whose symptoms are mild or remedied enough to appear unexceptional ("normal") to others. Although this is controversial, autistic individuals are often divided into those with an IQ less than 80 (referred to as having "low-functioning autism" or LFA), and those with an IQ over 80 (referred to as having "high-functioning autism" (HFA).<ref>[http://www.nimh.nih.gov/publicat/autism.cfm Autism Spectrum Disorders (Pervasive Developmental Disorders)] National Institute of Mental Health (2004) Retrieved 24 March, 2007</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.nas.org.uk/nas/jsp/polopoly.jsp?d=1049&a=3337|title=Facts on autism|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref><ref>A search of the National Institute of Health database [http://search2.google.cit.nih.gov/search?entqr=0&access=p&getfields=*&output=xml_no_dtd&sort=date%3AD%3AL%3Ad1&ie=UTF-8&btnG=Google+Search&btnG.y=3&btnG.y=0&client=NIMH_frontend&q=%22high+functioning%22+autism&btnG.x=30&btnG.x=0&ud=1&site=NIMH&oe=UTF-8&proxystylesheet=NIMH_frontend&ip=222.152.215.130&filter=0] March 24, 2007 showed 12 papers or studies that employed the distinction "high/higher functioning autism". A similar search of the PubMed database [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?CMD=Pager&DB=pubmed] elicited 486 articles and entries employing the term "high-functioning autism" published between June, 1975 and April, 2007</ref>) Low and high functioning are more generally applied to how well an individual can accomplish activities of daily living, rather than to [[IQ]]. The terms low and high functioning are controversial and not all autistics accept these labels.  Additionally, a review of the literature in 2005 questioned the validity of IQ testing of autistic people, noting the frequency of poor methodology in numerous studies that assumed or failed to demonstrate impaired cognitive functioning.<ref>Edelson, M.G. (2006). Are the majority of children with autism mentally retarded?: A systematic evaluation of the data. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 21, 66-83.  Reprint online at [http://www.willamette.edu/dept/comm/reprint/edelson/ http://www.willamette.edu/dept/comm/reprint/edelson/], retrieved 15 April 2007</ref><ref>"Many times, if the researchers had a child they couldn't test, they just assumed he or she was retarded and assigned a low IQ score."  ''Professor challenges autism assumption.''  The Oregonian, November 25, 2006.[http://www.oregonlive.com/news/oregonian/index.ssf?/base/news/1164421506110210.xml&coll=7].  Retrieved 2007-02-25.</ref>


This discrepancy can lead to confusion among service providers who equate IQ with functioning and may refuse to serve high-IQ autistic people who are severely compromised in their ability to perform daily living tasks, or may fail to recognize the intellectual potential of many autistic people who are considered LFA. For example, some professionals refuse to recognize autistics who can speak or write as being autistic at all, because they still think of autism as a communication disorder so severe that no speech or writing is possible.
This discrepancy can lead to confusion among service providers who equate IQ with functioning and may refuse to serve high-IQ autistic people who are severely compromised in their ability to perform daily living tasks, or may fail to recognize the intellectual potential of many autistic people who are considered LFA. For example, some professionals refuse to recognize autistics who can speak or write as being autistic at all, because they still think of autism as a communication disorder so severe that no speech or writing is possible.


As a consequence, many "high-functioning" autistic persons, and autistic people with a relatively high [[IQ]], are under diagnosed, thus making the claim that "autism implies retardation" self-fulfilling. The number of people diagnosed with LFA is not rising quite as sharply as HFA, indicating that at least part of the explanation for the apparent rise is probably better diagnostics. Many also think that ASD's are being over diagnosed: (1) because the growth in the number and complexity of symptoms associated with autism has increased the chances professionals will erroneously diagnose autism and (2) because the growth in services and therapies for autism has increased the number who falsely qualify for those often free services and therapies.
As a consequence, many "high-functioning" autistic persons, and autistic people with relatively high [[IQ]], are under-diagnosed, thus making the claim that "autism implies retardation" self-fulfilling. The number of people diagnosed with LFA is not rising quite as sharply as HFA, indicating that at least part of the explanation for the apparent rise is probably better diagnostics. Many also think that ASD's are being over diagnosed: (1) because the growth in the number and complexity of symptoms associated with autism has increased the chances professionals will erroneously diagnose autism and (2) because the growth in services and therapies for autism has increased the number who falsely qualify for those often free services and therapies.


===Asperger's and Kanner's syndromes===
===Asperger's Syndrome and Classic Autism Disorder===
[[image:Asperger_kl2.jpg|frame|right|Dr. [[Hans Asperger]] described a form of autism in the 1940s that later became known as [[Asperger syndrome]].]]
{{main|Asperger syndrome}}
{{main|Asperger syndrome}}
In the current ''Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders'' (DSM-IV-TR), the most significant difference between Autistic Disorder (also known as Kanner's syndrome) and Asperger's syndrome is that a diagnosis of the former includes the observation of "delays or abnormal functioning in at least one of the following areas, with onset prior to age 3 years: (1) social interaction, (2) language as used in social communication, or (3) symbolic or imaginative play",<ref>{{cite web | title=BehaveNet autism description | url=http://www.behavenet.com/capsules/disorders/autistic.htm | accessmonthday = July 30 | accessyear=2005 }}</ref> while a diagnosis of Asperger's syndrome observes "no clinically significant delay" in the latter two of these areas.<ref>{{cite web | title=BehaveNet aspergers description | url=http://www.behavenet.com/capsules/disorders/asperger.htm | accessmonthday = July 30 | accessyear=2005 }}</ref>
In the current ''Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders'' (DSM-IV-TR), the most significant difference between Autistic Disorder and Asperger's syndrome is that a diagnosis of the former includes the observation of "delays or abnormal functioning in at least one of the following areas, with onset prior to age 3 years: (1) social interaction, (2) language as used in social communication, or (3) symbolic or imaginative play",<ref>{{cite web | title=BehaveNet autism description | url=http://www.behavenet.com/capsules/disorders/autistic.htm | accessmonthday = July 30 | accessyear=2005 }}</ref> while a diagnosis of Asperger's syndrome observes "no clinically significant delay" in the latter two of these areas.<ref>{{cite web | title=BehaveNet aspergers description | url=http://www.behavenet.com/capsules/disorders/asperger.htm | accessmonthday = July 30 | accessyear=2005 }}</ref>


While the DSM-IV does not include level of intellectual functioning in the diagnosis, the fact that those with Asperger's syndrome tend to perform better than those with Kanner's autism has produced a popular conception that [[Asperger's syndrome]] is synonymous with "higher-functioning autism", or that it is a lesser [[disorder]] than autism. Similarly, there is a popular conception that autistic individuals with a high level of intellectual functioning in fact have Asperger's syndrome, or that both types are merely '[[geek]]s' with a medical label. The popular depiction of autism in the media has been of relatively severe cases (for example, as seen in the films ''[[Rain Man]]'' and ''[[Mercury Rising]]''), and in turn many close friends and relatives of those who have been diagnosed in the autistic spectrum choose to speak of their loved ones as having Asperger's syndrome rather than autism.
While the DSM-IV does not include level of intellectual functioning in the diagnosis, the fact that those with Asperger's syndrome tend to perform better than those with classic autism has produced a popular conception that [[Asperger's syndrome]] is synonymous with "higher-functioning autism", or that it is a lesser disorder than autism.  


The extent to which someone with higher functioning autism or Asperger's syndrome may excel is theoretically quite high. For example, [[Henry Cavendish]], one of history's foremost scientists, may have been autistic. [[George Wilson]], a notable chemist and physician, wrote a book about Cavendish entitled, "''The Life of the Honourable Henry Cavendish''", published in 1851. From Wilson's detailed description it seems that while Cavendish may have exhibited many classic signs of autism, he nevertheless had an extraordinary mind.<ref>[http://www.neurological.org.nz/html/article.php?documentCode=26 "Henry Cavendish: An early case of Asperger's syndrome?"] Oliver Sachs (Albert Einstein College of Medicine) Neurological Foundation of New Zealand (Reprinted with permission from the American Neurological Association)</ref>
The extent to which someone with higher functioning autism or Asperger's syndrome may excel is theoretically quite high. For example, [[Henry Cavendish]], one of history's foremost scientists, may have been autistic. George Wilson, a notable chemist and physician, wrote a book about Cavendish entitled, "''The Life of the Honourable Henry Cavendish''", published in 1851. From Wilson's detailed description it seems that while Cavendish may have exhibited many classic signs of autism, he nevertheless had an extraordinary mind.<ref>[http://www.neurological.org.nz/html/article.php?documentCode=26 "Henry Cavendish: An early case of Asperger's syndrome?"] Oliver Sachs (Albert Einstein College of Medicine) Neurological Foundation of New Zealand (Reprinted with permission from the American Neurological Association)</ref>


===Autism as a spectrum disorder===
===Autism as a spectrum disorder===
{{details|Autistic spectrum}}
 
Another view of these disorders is that they are on a continuum known as [[autistic spectrum]] disorders. Autism spectrum disorder is an increasingly popular term that refers to a broad definition of autism including the classic form of the disorder as well as closely related conditions such as PDD-NOS and Asperger's syndrome. Although the classic form of autism can be easily distinguished from other forms of autism spectrum disorder, the terms are often used interchangeably.
Another view of these disorders is that they are on a continuum known as [[autistic spectrum]] disorders. Autism spectrum disorder is an increasingly popular term that refers to a broad definition of autism including the classic form of the disorder as well as closely related conditions such as PDD-NOS (Pervasive Developmental Disorders-Not Otherwise Specified) and Asperger's syndrome. Although the classic form of autism can be easily distinguished from other forms of autism spectrum disorder, the terms are often used interchangeably.


A related continuum, [[Sensory Integration Dysfunction]], involves how well humans integrate the information they receive from their senses. Autism, Asperger's syndrome, and Sensory Integration Dysfunction are all closely related and overlap.
A related continuum, [[Sensory Integration Dysfunction]], involves how well humans integrate the information they receive from their senses. Autism, Asperger's syndrome, and Sensory Integration Dysfunction are all closely related and overlap.
Line 147: Line 117:


==Epidemiology==
==Epidemiology==
{{further|[[Frequency of autism]] and [[Autism (incidence)]].}}
;Gender differences
There is not a clear-cut ratio of incidence between males and females. Studies have found much higher prevalence in males at the high-functioning end of the spectrum, while the ratios appear to be closer to 1:1 at the low-functioning end.<ref>Volkmar, F.R., Szatmari, P. & Sparro, S.S. (1993). Sex differences in pervasive developmental disorders. ''Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(4),'' 579-591.</ref> In addition, a study published in 2006 suggested that males over 40 are more likely than younger males to parent a child with autism, and that the ratio of autism incidence in males and females is closer to 1:1 with older fathers.<ref>{{cite journal | last=Reichenberg | first=Abraham | coauthors=et al. |
title=Advancing Paternal Age and Autism | journal=Archives of General Psychiatry | volume=63 | issue=9 | date=September 2006 | pages=1026-1032 | format = [[PDF]]|url=http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/cgi/reprint/63/9/1026}}</ref><ref>{{cite news | last=Vince | first=Gaia | title=Older dads boost risk of autistic children | date=September 2006 | publisher=NewScientist.com | url=http://www.newscientist.com/article.ns?id=dn9998}} Layman's synopsis of the A. Reichenberg article</ref>


;Reported increase with time
'''Gender differences'''
[[Image:autismnocgraph.png|right|thumb|400px|The number of reported cases of autism increased dramatically over a decade. Statistics in graph from the [[National Center for Health Statistics]].]]


There was a worldwide increase in reported cases of autism over the decade to 2006. There are several theories about the apparent sudden increase.
The ratio of incidence between males and females (as demonstrated in the scientific literature) is ambiguous. Studies have found much higher prevalence in males at the high-functioning end of the spectrum, while the ratios appear to be closer to 1:1 at the low-functioning end.<ref>Volkmar, F.R., Szatmari, P. & Sparro, S.S. (1993). Sex differences in pervasive developmental disorders. ''Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(4),'' 579-591.</ref> In addition, a study published in 2006 suggested that males over 40 are more likely than younger males to parent a child with autism, and that the ratio of autism incidence in males and females is closer to 1:1 with older fathers.<ref>{{cite journal | last=Reichenberg | first=Abraham | coauthors=et al. |
 
title=Advancing Paternal Age and Autism | journal=Archives of General Psychiatry | volume=63 | issue=9 | date=September 2006 | pages=1026-1032 | format = [[PDF]]|url=http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/cgi/reprint/63/9/1026}}</ref><ref>{{cite news | last=Vince | first=Gaia | title=Older dads boost risk of autistic children | date=September 2006 | publisher=NewScientist.com | url=http://www.newscientist.com/article.ns?id=dn9998}} Layman's synopsis of the A. Reichenberg article</ref>
Many epidemiologists argue that the rise in the incidence of autism in the United States is largely attributable to a broadening of the diagnostic concept, reclassifications, public awareness, and the incentive to receive federally mandated services.<ref>{{cite journal | last=Jick | first=H | coauthors=JA Kaye |
title=Epidemiology and causes of autism | journal=Pharmacotherapy | volume=23 | issue=12 | date=December 2003 | pages=1524-30}}</ref><!-- describe it, do not give a reference --> However, some authors indicate that the existence of an as yet unidentified contributing environmental risk factor cannot be ruled out.<ref>{{cite journal | last=Rutter | first=M | title=Incidence of autism spectrum disorders: changes over time and their meaning | journal=Acta Paediatrica | volume=94 | issue=1 | date=January 2005 | pages=2-15}}</ref> On the other hand, a widely-cited pilot study conducted in California by the UC Davis [[M.I.N.D. Institute]] ([[17 October]] [[2002]]), reported that the increase in autism in California is real, even after accounting for changes to diagnostic criteria.<ref>{{cite web | title=Report to the Legislature on the Principal Findings of the Epidemiology of Autism in California Pilot Study | url=http://www.ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/mindinstitute/newsroom/study_final.pdf | accessmonthday = September 18 | accessyear=2006}}</ref>


The question of whether the rise in incidence is real or an artifact of improved diagnosis and a broader concept of autism remains controversial. Dr. Chris Johnson, a professor of pediatrics at the University of Texas Health Sciences Center at [[San Antonio]] and co-chair of the [[American Academy of Pediatrics]] Autism Expert Panel, sums up the state of the issue by saying, "There is a chance we're seeing a true rise, but right now I do not think anybody can answer that question for sure." ([[Newsweek]] reference below).<ref>{{cite news | last=Stenson | first=Jacqueline | title=As autism cases soar, a search for clues | date=[[24 February]] [[2005]] | publisher=Newsweek | url=http://www.msnbc.msn.com/id/6947652/}}</ref>
'''Reported increase with time'''


The answer to this question has significant ramifications on the direction of research, since a real increase would focus more attention (and research funding) on the search for environmental factors, while the alternative would focus more attention to genetics. On the other hand, it is conceivable that certain environmental factors (such as chemicals, infections, medicines, [[vaccine]]s, diet and societal changes) may have a particular impact on people with a specific genetic constitution.
There was a worldwide increase in reported cases of autism over the decade leading up to 2006. There are several theories about this apparent sudden increase.


One of the more popular theories is that there is a connection between "geekdom" and autism. This is hinted, for instance, by a ''Wired Magazine'' article in 2001 entitled "The [[Geek]] Syndrome", which is a point argued by many in the autism rights movement.<ref>{{cite news | last=Silberman | first=Steve|title=The Geek Syndrome|date=December 2001|publisher=Wired|url=http://www.wired.com/wired/archive/9.12/aspergers_pr.html}}</ref> This article, many professionals assert, is just one example of the media's application of mental disease labels to what is actually variant normal behavior&mdash;they argue that shyness, lack of athletic ability or social skills, and intellectual interests, even when they seem unusual to others, are not in themselves signs of autism or Asperger's syndrome. Others assert that children who in the past would have simply been accepted as a little different or even labeled 'gifted' are now being labeled with mental disease diagnoses. See [[clinomorphism]] for further discussion of this issue.
Many epidemiologists argue that the rise in the incidence of autism in the United States is largely attributable to a broadening of the diagnostic concept, earlier diagnosis (thereby accounting for those cases which are actually diagnosed as early as one year of age)<ref>[http://www.nimh.nih.gov/autismiacc/californiaautism.cfm Infants at Risk of Autism: A Longitudinal Study] STAART Network Centers: University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), National Institute of Mental Health</ref> reclassifications, public awareness, and the incentive to receive federally mandated services.<ref>{{cite journal | last=Jick | first=H | coauthors=JA Kaye |
title=Epidemiology and causes of autism | journal=Pharmacotherapy | volume=23 | issue=12 | date=December 2003 | pages=1524-30}}</ref> However, some authors indicate that the existence of an as yet unidentified contributing environmental risk factor cannot be ruled out.<ref>{{cite journal | last=Rutter | first=M | title=Incidence of autism spectrum disorders: changes over time and their meaning | journal=Acta Paediatrica | volume=94 | issue=1 | date=January 2005 | pages=2-15}}</ref> A widely-cited pilot study conducted in California by the UC Davis M.I.N.D. Institute (17 October, 2002), reported that the increase in autism in California is real, even after accounting for changes to diagnostic criteria.<ref>{{cite web | title=Report to the Legislature on the Principal Findings of the Epidemiology of Autism in California Pilot Study | url=http://www.ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/mindinstitute/newsroom/study_final.pdf | accessmonthday = September 18 | accessyear=2006}}</ref>


Due to the recent publicity surrounding autism and autistic spectrum disorders, an increasing number of adults are choosing to seek diagnoses of high-functioning autism or Asperger's syndrome in light of symptoms they currently experience or experienced during childhood. Since the cause of autism is thought to be at least partly genetic, a proportion of these adults seek their own diagnosis specifically as follow-up to their children's diagnoses. Because autism falls into the [[pervasive developmental disorder]] category, an individual's symptoms must have been present before age seven in order to make a strict [[differential diagnosis]].
The question of whether the rise in incidence is real or an artifact of improved diagnosis and a broader concept of autism remains controversial. Dr. Chris Johnson, a professor of pediatrics at the University of Texas Health Sciences Center at San Antonio and co-chair of the American Academy of Pediatrics Autism Expert Panel, sums up the state of the issue by saying, "There is a chance we're seeing a true rise, but right now I do not think anybody can answer that question for sure." <ref>{{cite news | last=Stenson | first=Jacqueline | title=As autism cases soar, a search for clues | date=24 February 2005 | publisher=Newsweek | url=http://www.msnbc.msn.com/id/6947652/}}</ref>


==Treatment==
==Treatment==
{{main|Autism therapies}}
[[Image:Bushcombatautism.jpg|U.S. President [[George W. Bush]] signing a piece of legislation aimed to combat autism |right|thumb]]
 
Autism is sometimes considered to be untreatable, although this point has been disputed.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.nationalautismconference.org/|title=National autism conference claiming it to be incurable|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.sci.fi/~biopteri/|title=Argument against autism being incurable|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref> There is a broad array of '''autism therapies''' with various goals, e.g. improving health and well-being, emotional problems, difficulties with communication and learning, and sensory problems for people with autism. The [[efficacy]] of each approach varies greatly from person to person.
 
[[Applied Behavior Analysis]] (ABA)<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.ascribe.org/cgi-bin/behold.pl?ascribeid=20050805.160552&time=16|title=Webpage discussing the effectiveness of ABA|accessdate=2007-01-25}}</ref> is an approach in which tasks are systematically reduced to component parts and then reconstructed through repetition and [[positive reinforcement]]. The approach also attempts to identify and analyze behaviors that are harmful or that interfere with learning to ensure they are not reinforced. Ultimately the goal is to help children to succeed and become independent socially and academically.
 
==Causes==
{{main|Causes of autism}}


The '''causes and etiology of autism''' are areas of debate and controversy; there is currently no consensus, and researchers are studying a wide range of possible genetic and environmental causes. Since autistic individuals are all somewhat different from one another, there are likely multiple "causes" that interact with each other in subtle and complex ways, and thus give slightly differing outcomes in each individual. Two environmental theories include the impact of vaccines on the immune system (of which a statistically significant link has never been found despite many attempts; see the [[Causes of autism#Vaccine theory|vaccine theory]] sub-heading in the Causes of autism page for a more extensive treatment) and a more recent theory relating autism to high levels of television viewing while young.<ref>{{cite web|url=\http://www.johnson.cornell.edu/faculty/profiles/Waldman/AUTISM-WALDMAN-NICHOLSON-ADILOV.pdf|title="Does television cause autism" at johnson.cornell.edu/faculty/profiles/waldman/autpaper.html|accessdate=2007-02-26}}</ref>
The efficacy of treating autism is disputed. There is a broad array of proposed autism therapies with various goals, e.g. improving health and well-being, emotional problems, difficulties with communication and learning, and sensory problems for people with autism, but the effectiveness of these approaches is not clear.  


Research claims also link autism with abnormal blood vessel function, and [[oxidative stress]]. This line of research may lead to new medical therapies.<ref>{{cite web | title=Penn Researchers Find Link Between Autism and Abnormal Blood-Vessel Function and Oxidative Stress | url=http://www.uphs.upenn.edu/news/News_Releases/aug06/autbldvsl.htm | accessmonthday = August 15 | accessyear=2006 }}</ref>
[[Antipsychotic agent|Antipsychotics]] are used to manage disruptive and violent behavior, such as irritability, tantrums and violence.  [[Risperidone]] (trade name Risperdal) is the only anti-psychotic agent currently approved by the U.S. [[Food and Drug Administration]] to treat children and adolescents with autism.<ref name="pmid18172517">{{cite journal |author=Posey DJ, Stigler KA, Erickson CA, McDougle CJ |title=Antipsychotics in the treatment of autism |journal=J. Clin. Invest. |volume=118 |issue=1 |pages=6–14 |year=2008 |pmid=18172517 |doi=10.1172/JCI32483}}</ref> Risperidone benefited patients in a [[randomized controlled trial]].<ref name="pmid12151468">{{cite journal| author=McCracken JT, McGough J, Shah B, Cronin P, Hong D, Aman MG et al.| title=Risperidone in children with autism and serious behavioral problems. | journal=N Engl J Med | year= 2002 | volume= 347 | issue= 5 | pages= 314-21 | pmid=12151468
| url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&tool=clinical.uthscsa.edu/cite&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks&id=12151468 | doi=10.1056/NEJMoa013171 }} </ref>


===Physiology and neurology===
==Physiology and neurology==
Autism appears to involve a greater amount of the brain than previously thought.<ref>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/news/releases/autism_affects_brain.cfm "Study Provides Evidence That Autism Affects Functioning of Entire Brain-Previous View Held Autism Limited to Communication, Social Behavior, and Reasoning"] National Institutes of Health, (Aug, 16, 2006)</ref> A study of 112 children (56 with autism and 56 without), published in the ''Journal of Child Neuropsychology'', found those with autism to have more problems with complex tasks, such as tying their shoelaces or writing, which suggests that many areas of the brain are involved.<ref name="Flaw">[http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/4794085.stm “Autism 'affects all of the brain'”] BBC News (2006) Wednesday, 16 August</ref>
Autism appears to involve a greater amount of the brain than previously thought.<ref>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/news/releases/autism_affects_brain.cfm "Study Provides Evidence That Autism Affects Functioning of Entire Brain-Previous View Held Autism Limited to Communication, Social Behavior, and Reasoning"] National Institutes of Health, (Aug, 16, 2006)</ref> A study of 112 children (56 with autism and 56 without), published in the ''Journal of Child Neuropsychology'', found those with autism to have more problems with complex tasks, such as tying their shoelaces or writing, which suggests that many areas of the brain are involved.<ref name="Flaw">[http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/4794085.stm “Autism 'affects all of the brain'”] BBC News (2006) Wednesday, 16 August</ref>
Children with autism performed simple tasks as well as or better than those without. In tests of visual and spatial skills, autistic children did well at finding small objects in complex pictures (e.g., finding the character Waldo in "[[Where's Waldo]]" pictures).
Children with autism performed simple tasks as well as or better than those without. In tests of visual and spatial skills, autistic children did well at finding small objects in complex pictures (e.g., finding the character Waldo in "Where's Waldo" pictures).
However, they found it difficult to tell the difference between similar-looking people. Children with autism tended to do well in spelling and grammar, but found it much more difficult to understand complex speech, such as idioms or similes when the meaning of the phrase is figurative. They would, for example, not understand that "He kicked the bucket" meant someone had died, or were likely to actually hop if told to "hop to it".
However, they found it difficult to tell the difference between similar-looking people. Children with autism tended to do well in spelling and grammar, but found it much more difficult to understand complex speech, such as idioms or similes when the meaning of the phrase is figurative. They would, for example, not understand that "He kicked the bucket" meant someone had died, or were likely to actually hop if told to "hop to it".
The inference from this research, according to researchers at the Pittsburgh School of Medicine, is that "These findings show that you cannot compartmentalize autism. It's much more complex.”<ref name="Flaw"> This should be blank </ref>


The research from this perspective has a number of implications:
The research from this perspective has a number of implications:
Line 196: Line 152:
*Autism may not be primarily a disorder of social interaction; research must now take into account non-social aspects.
*Autism may not be primarily a disorder of social interaction; research must now take into account non-social aspects.


A possible explanation for the characteristics of the syndrome is a variation in the way the brain itself reacts to sensory input and how parts of the brain then handle the information. An electroencephalographic (EEG) study of 36 adults (half of whom had autism) at [[Washington University in St. Louis]] found that adults with autism show differences in the manner in which neural activity is coordinated. The implication seems to be that there is poor internal communication between different areas of the brain. ([[Electroencephalograph]]s, or EEGs, measure the activity of brain cells.)
A possible explanation for the characteristics of the syndrome is a variation in the way the brain itself reacts to sensory input and how parts of the brain then handle the information. An electroencephalographic (EEG) study of 36 adults (half of whom had autism) at Washington University in St. Louis found that adults with autism show differences in the manner in which neural activity is coordinated. The implication seems to be that there is poor internal communication between different areas of the brain. (Electroencephalographs, or EEGs, measure the activity of brain cells.)


The study indicated that there were abnormal patterns in the way the brain cells were connected in the [[temporal lobe]] of the brain. (The temporal lobe deals with language.) These abnormal patterns would seem to indicate inefficient and inconsistent communication inside the brain of autistic people.<ref>[http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/6037836.stm “Clue to flaws in autistic brain”] BBC News (2006) Saturday, 14 October</ref>
The Wash. U. study indicated that there were abnormal patterns in the way the brain cells were connected in the [[temporal lobe]] of the brain. (The temporal lobe deals with language.) These abnormal patterns would seem to indicate inefficient and inconsistent communication inside the brain of autistic people.<ref>[http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/6037836.stm “Clue to flaws in autistic brain”] BBC News (2006) Saturday, 14 October</ref>


Studies in [[neuropathology]]<ref>[ http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/sos_autism/sub6.cfm Pathophysiology of Autism: Brain Mechanisms] M. Denckla (08/15/2006) National Institute of Mental Health</ref> indicate abnormalities in the [[amygdala]], [[hippocampus]], [[septum]], [[mamillary bodies]], [[limbic system]],and the [[cerebellum]].
Studies in [[neuropathology]] indicate abnormalities in the [[amygdala]], [[hippocampus]], [[septum]], [[mamillary bodies]], [[limbic system]],and the [[cerebellum]]. <ref name=NIMH-Patho>[http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/sos_autism/sub6.cfm Pathophysiology of Autism: Brain Mechanisms] M. Denckla (08/15/2006) National Institute of Mental Health</ref><ref>Hardan, A., Minshew, N., Mallikarjuhn, M., Keshavan, M. (2001). Brain Volume in Autism. ''Journal of Child Neurology, 16'', 421-424. [http://www.wpic.pitt.edu/research/CeFAR/PDF/Brain%20Volume%20in%20autism.pdf]</ref>
*Autistic brains are slightly larger and heavier and a larger than normal head circumference is commonly noted.  
*Autistic brains are slightly larger and heavier and a larger than normal head circumference is commonly noted.  
*In the limbic system, there is an excess of cells and they are too small. The neurons themselves appear to be underdeveloped. Dendritic trees which provide the basis for connections between neurons are truncated (i.e. shortened).  
*In the limbic system, there is an excess of cells and they are too small. The neurons themselves appear to be underdeveloped. Dendritic trees which provide the basis for connections between neurons are truncated (i.e. shortened).  
*In the cerebellum, [[purkinje cells]] are widely affected. The anatomic differences correlate to the curtailment of development earlier than 30 weeks gestation. In other words, the development of the cells appears to have stopped at some time before the 30th week in utero
*In the cerebellum, [[purkinje cells]] are widely affected. The anatomic differences correlate to the curtailment of development earlier than 30 weeks gestation. In other words, the development of the cells appears to have stopped at some time before the 30th week in utero
*An enlarged [[third ventricle]] of the brain appears to accompany autism in those who are non-mentally retarded, but the reasons for this and its effects are still unknown.<ref>Hardan, A., Minshew, N., Mallikarjuhn, M., Keshavan, M. (2001). Brain Volume in Autism. ''Journal of Child Neurology, 16'', 421-424. [http://www.wpic.pitt.edu/research/CeFAR/PDF/Brain%20Volume%20in%20autism.pdf]</ref>
*An enlarged [[third ventricle]] of the brain appears to accompany autism in those who are non-mentally retarded, but the reasons for this and its effects are still unknown.
Research has not yet established exactly what is specific to autism and what may be seen in other disorders however.<ref>[ http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/pubs/sos_autism/sub6.cfm Pathophysiology of Autism: Brain Mechanisms] M. Denckla (08/15/2006) National Institute of Mental Health</ref>
 
Research has not yet established exactly what is specific to autism and what may be seen in other disorders however.<ref name=NIMH-Patho/>


Individuals with autism are also far more likely to develop [[epilepsy]] than would otherwise be expected (estimated 10-30% incidence).
Individuals with autism are also far more likely to develop [[epilepsy]] than would otherwise be expected (estimated 10-30% incidence).
<ref>[https://www.epilepsyfoundation.org/epilepsyusa/yebeh/upload/autismandepilepsy.pdf Autism and epilepsy: Cause, consequence, comorbidity, or coincidence?],  
<ref>[https://www.epilepsyfoundation.org/epilepsyusa/yebeh/upload/autismandepilepsy.pdf Autism and epilepsy: Cause, consequence, comorbidity, or coincidence?], Lidia Gabis, John Pomeroy, and Mary R. Andriola, in Epilepsy & Behavior 7 (2005) 652–656</ref>
Lidia Gabis, John Pomeroy, and Mary R. Andriola, in  
Epilepsy & Behavior 7 (2005) 652–656</ref>


===Genetic component===
===Mirror neurons===
{{Main|Heritability of autism}}
A theory featuring [[mirror neurons]]<ref>Oberman L., Hubbard E., McCleery J., Altschuler E., Ramachandran V., Pineda J. (2006). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders, ''Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2)'', 190-198.</ref><ref>Dapretto, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. ''Nature Neuroscience, 9(1)'', 28-30.</ref> states that autism may involve a dysfunction of specialized neurons in the brain that should activate when observing other people. In typically-developing people, these mirror neurons are thought to perhaps play a major part in [[social learning]] and general comprehension of the actions of others.
Genetic influence comprises a significant aspect of research in the causes of autism.<ref>{http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=384967/ Presence of Large Deletions in Kindreds with Autism] Yu C., Dawson G., Munson J., D'Souza I., Osterling J., Estes A., Leutenegger A., Flodman P., Smith M., Raskind W., Spence M., McMahon W., Wijsman E., Schellenberg G. (2002). ''American Journal of Human Genetics, 71'', 100-115.</ref> A large database showing theoretical links between autism and genetic [[loci]] summarises research indicating that the genetic influence may extend to every human [[chromosome]].<ref>[http://projects.tcag.ca/autism/ The Autism Chromosome Rearrangement Database] Retrieved [[November 19]] [[2006]]</ref> It has been observed in one twin-study in Britain that there was about a 60% concordance rate for autism in [[monozygotic]] (identical) [[twin]]s,<ref>Bailey A., Le Couteur A., Gottesman I., Bolton P., Simonoff E., Yuzda E., Rutter M. (1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. ''Psychological Medicine, 25'', 63–77.</ref> while [[dizygotic]] (non-identical) twins and other siblings comparatively exhibited about 4% concordance rates.<ref>Bolton P., MacDonald H., Pickles A., Rios P., Goode S., Crowson M., Bailey A., Rutter M. (1994). A case-control family history study of autism. ''Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35'', 877–900.</ref> Some research posits that the chances that an identical twin of an autistic person will also be autistic are 85-90%.<ref>[http://www.physorg.com/news78151666.html When nerve cells can’t make contact] Physorg.co, (2006) Sept. 22 Retrieved March 3, 2007</ref> The increased probabilities of siblings having autism has been calculated at about 35-fold more than normal.<ref>[http://www.nih.gov/news/pr/oct2006/nimh-17.htm Gene Linked to Autism in Families with More Than One Affected Child] National Institutes of Health News(2006) Oct. Retrieved March 3, 2007</ref>


Accompanying impairments are also a common feature of autism. Some people with autism also have [[gastrointestinal]], [[immunological]] or [[neurological]] symptoms in addition to behavioral impairments. These associated complexes have also lead to the search for specific genetic connections and helped to focus on reasonable genetic implications.<ref>[http://www.nih.gov/news/pr/oct2006/nimh-17.htm Gene Linked to Autism in Families with More Than One Affected Child] National Institutes of Health News(2006) Oct. Retrienved March 3, 2007</ref>
==Causes==


Since genes provide the information for processes and structure at the level of the [[Cell (biology)|cell]] and its components during the growth and development of a human as well as maintenance during life, [[gene]] [[mutations]] (altered versions) and deletions (complete absence of genetic material) and possibly extra copies of genes would mean that the causes of autism begin very early. If a mutated gene fails to perform properly, then [[Cell (biology)|cells]], [[proteins]], [[enzymes]] and other crucial aspects of normal function may be significantly altered and operate incorrectly. Deletions could mean the complete absence of a sequence of events due to missing proteins or cell components for example. These genetic alterations and deletions will simply bring about a changed structure or process which effects a great many other needed structures and processes.
The causes and etiology of autism are uncertain. Possible genetic and environmental causes are a common focus in published literature. Since autistic individuals are all somewhat different from one another, multiple "causes" have been proposed that interact with each other in subtle and complex ways, and thus give slightly differing outcomes in each individual.  


Another important aspect of research in genetic factors is environmental effects and the incidence of autism. During the lifetime of a person, gene mutations and deletions may be environmentally triggered or exacerbated. Conversely, it may also be that environment will not be a factor and nothing will change the autism characteristics. For autism, the answer to these possible explanations is still being researched and there is evidence that both may be true simply because there is more than one way a person may develop autism.
Research claims also link autism with abnormal blood vessel function, and oxidative stress. <ref>{{cite web | title=Penn Researchers Find Link Between Autism and Abnormal Blood-Vessel Function and Oxidative Stress | url=http://www.uphs.upenn.edu/news/News_Releases/aug06/autbldvsl.htm | accessmonthday = August 15 | accessyear=2006 }}</ref>


====Deletions and Mutations====
===Genetic component===
Deleted genes have been noted as a probable influence or cause in autism.<ref>[http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=384967/ Presence of Large Deletions in Kindreds with Autism] Yu et al (2002) Am J Hum Genet.  July; 71(1): 100–115.</ref> By locating specific missing genetic material the significance may be that specific genetic sites for autism controlling or causing autism  (autism susceptibility alleles) may be located precisely. Another significant aspect of this research is that these deletions of genetic material indicate that autism may be established in some cases during meiosis (error-prone meiosis model) and this places the genesis of autism in some at the very beginning of life.
 
One very important question in this line of research is whether or not gene deletions are a cause or consequence of autism-susceptibility [[loci]] located elsewhere in the [[chromosomes]].
 
Gene mutations may mean a gene does not function at all or does not function in the normal way. Since genes direct how the body grows and develops, mutations, like deletions, will effect a person at the most basic levels.
 
Mutation and deletion effects have been delineated in numerous research publications.<ref>[http://www.jcb.org/cgi/content/full/167/5/945 The neuronal scaffold protein Shank3 mediates signaling and biological function of the receptor tyrosine kinase Ret in epithelial cells] G. Schuetz et al (2004) Journal of Cell Biology. Volume 167, Number 5, 945-952</ref><ref>[http://www.cureautismnow.org/site/apps/nl/content2.asp?c=bhLOK2PILuF&b=1289189&ct=3422589  Researchers Find Rare Mutations in SHANK3 Gene Are Associated with Autism] Synopsis of information from Nature Genetics (January 2007). Cure Autism Now. Retrieved March 3, 2007</ref><ref>[http://synapse-web.org/lab/harris/Lecture10-11/sld069.htm Neuroligins] Kristen Harris (2001) Cell adhesion at synapses Synapse Web, Laboratory of Synapse Structure and Function. Human Brain Project. National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of Drug Abuse</ref>
<ref> [http://stke.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/abstract/2005/265/tw8 Neuroligins Organize Excitatory and Inhibitory Postsynaptic Membrane] E. R. Graf et al, (2004). Cell Issue 119, pages 1013-1026</ref>.
<ref>[http://www.autismvox.com/neuroligin-mutations-as-a-cause-of-autism/ Neuroligin Mutations as a Cause of Autism] K. Chew Autism Vox Retreived March 3, 2007</ref>
 
Correlated characteristics include global developmental delay, mild to severe delay of speech, social communication disorders and cognitive abilities, autistic like behaviour, high tolerance of pain, and repetitive mannerisms (e.g. chewing or mouthing). <ref>[http://www.orpha.net/data/patho/GB/uk-22q13.pdf Deletion 22q13 Syndrome] M.C Phelan (2003) Orphanet.com</ref>
 
Gene interaction may also complicate the causes leading to multiple genetic origins of autism<ref>[http://www.cureautismnow.org/site/apps/nl/content2.asp?c=bhLOK2PILuF&b=1289189&ct=3422589  Researchers Find Rare Mutations in SHANK3 Gene Are Associated with Autism] Synopsis of information from Nature Genetics (January 2007). Cure Autism Now. Retrieved March 3, 2007</ref>
<ref>[http://www.orpha.net/data/patho/GB/uk-22q13.pdf Deletion 22q13 Syndrome] M.C Phelan (2003) Orphanet.com</ref>, In a cascade like effect, when a gene loci is altered or omitted, others are effected due to change in interaction between genes and/or their functions.
 
Though not present in all individuals with autism, these mutations and deletions hold potential to point the way to more the genetic components of spectrum disorders.<ref>December 2006; doi:10.1038/ng1933, Nature Genetics)</ref> The research also advanced basic understanding in the genetic architecture of the genome of autistic individuals and will help in focusing future research.
 
One practical aspect of this type of research may be the development of a test that would confirm the autism diagnosis in children exhibiting symptoms and identify families who carry genetic defects that could be inherited by their children.
 
====Mirror neurons====
A theory featuring [[mirror neurons]]<ref>Oberman L., Hubbard E., McCleery J., Altschuler E., Ramachandran V., Pineda J. (2006). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders, ''Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2)'', 190-198.</ref><ref>Dapretto, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. ''Nature Neuroscience, 9(1)'', 28-30.</ref> states that autism may involve a dysfunction of specialized neurons in the brain that should activate when observing other people. In typically-developing people, these mirror neurons are thought to perhaps play a major part in [[social learning]] and general comprehension of the actions of others.
 
==Sociology==
Due to the complexity of autism, there are many facets of [[sociology]] that need to be considered when discussing it, such as the culture which has evolved from autistic persons connecting and communicating with one another. In addition, there are several subgroups forming within the autistic community, sometimes in strong opposition to one another.
 
===Community and politics===
{{further|[[Autistic community]] and [[Autism rights movement]].}}
 
Curing autism is a very highly [[controversial]] and [[politicized issue]]. What some call the "autistic community" has splintered into several strands. Some seek a cure for autism - sometimes dubbed by ''pro-cure''. Others do not desire a "cure", because they point out that autism is a way of life rather than a "disease", and as such resist it. They are sometimes dubbed ''anti-cure''. Many more may have views between these two. Recently, with scientists learning more about autism and possibly coming closer to effective remedies, some members of the "anti-cure" movement [[Autistic community#Declaration from the autism community|sent a letter to the United Nations]] demanding to be treated as a [[minority group]] rather than a group with a [[mental disability]] or disease.<ref>{{cite web | title=autistics.org: The REAL Voice of Autism (See above) | url=http://www.autistics.org | accessmonthday = December 11 | accessyear=2005 }}</ref>


There are many resources available for autistic people. Because many autistics find it easier to communicate online than in person, a large number of these resources are online. In addition, successful autistic adults, using their own experience in developing coping strategies and/or interacting with society, are often involved at the community level with children with autism, using their own experience in developing coping strategies and/or interacting with society.
'''Extent of genetic origins of autism spectrum disorders:''' Genetic influence comprises a significant aspect of research in the causes of autism.<ref name=AJHG-Deletions>[http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=384967/ Presence of Large Deletions in Kindreds with Autism] Yu C., Dawson G., Munson J., D'Souza I., Osterling J., Estes A., Leutenegger A., Flodman P., Smith M., Raskind W., Spence M., McMahon W., Wijsman E., Schellenberg G. (2002). ''American Journal of Human Genetics, 71'', 100-115.</ref>


The year 2002 was declared Autism Awareness Year in the [[United Kingdom]] (see [[Autistic Community]] article).
More than one hundred different genes have been implicated in the causes of autism spectrum disorders (ASD). One type of genetic origin are “glitches” or small changes in DNA that are not genetic mutations, per se, but copy number variations (CNVs) which are extra copies or missing stretches of DNA. (In one case study a child with Asperger’s Syndrome was found to be missing a chain of 27 genes.) The extent of the possible genetic origins indicate that the abilities in social interaction and behavior, social and physical composure and impulse control are scattered throughout the human genome, implying that small genetic alterations, copies or deletions anywhere can have broad effects<ref name=Reuters-glitches>[http://www.nlm.nih.gov/medlineplus/news/fullstory_46617.html DNA "glitches" tied to autism, researchers say] Reuters Health (2007) March 15. Retrieved March 24, 2007</ref>


===Culture===
A large database showing theoretical links between autism and genetic [[loci]] summarises research indicating that the genetic influence may extend to every human [[chromosome]].<ref>[http://projects.tcag.ca/autism/ The Autism Chromosome Rearrangement Database] Retrieved November 19, 2006</ref> It has been observed in one twin-study in Britain that there was about a 60% concordance rate for autism in monozygotic (identical) twins,<ref>Bailey A., Le Couteur A., Gottesman I., Bolton P., Simonoff E., Yuzda E., Rutter M. (1995).
{{Main|Autistic culture}}


With the recent increases in autism recognition and new approaches to educating and socializing autistics, an ''autistic culture'' has begun to develop. Similar to [[deaf culture]], autistic culture is based on a more accepting belief that autism is a unique way of being and not a disorder to be cured. There are some commonalities which are specific to autism in general as a culture, not just "autistic culture".
'''Inheritability:''' Evidence of autism as a genetic disorder is supported by evidence from twin studies. In a British twin study. ''Psychological Medicine, 25'', 63–77.</ref> while dizygotic (non-identical) twins and other siblings comparatively exhibited about 4% concordance rates.<ref>Bolton P., MacDonald H., Pickles A., Rios P., Goode S., Crowson M., Bailey A., Rutter M. (1994). A case-control family history study of autism. ''Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35'', 877–900.</ref> Some research posits that the chances that an identical twin of an autistic person will also be autistic are 85-90%.<ref>[http://www.physorg.com/news78151666.html When nerve cells can’t make contact] Physorg.co, (2006) Sept. 22 Retrieved March 3, 2007</ref> The increased probabilities of siblings having autism has been calculated at about 35-fold more than normal.<ref name=NIHNews>[http://www.nih.gov/news/pr/oct2006/nimh-17.htm Gene Linked to Autism in Families with More Than One Affected Child] National Institutes of Health News(2006) Oct. Retrieved March 3, 2007</ref>


It is a common misperception that autistic people do not marry; many do seek out close relationships and marry. Often, they marry another autistic, although this is not always the case. Autistic people are often attracted to other autistic people due to shared interests or obsessions, but more often than not the attraction is due to simple compatibility with personality types, the same as for non-autistics. Autistics who communicate have explained that companionship is as important to autistics as it is to anyone else.
Generational links, or familial heritability may, however, be less prevalent than some research to date implies. Spontaneous changes in DNA are much more common than is usual in other diseases with a genetic component. The sporadic form of the disease accounts for about 90% of affected individuals.
<ref name=Reuters-glitches/>


It is also a common misperception that autistic people live away from other people, such as in a [[rural]] area rather than an [[urban area]]; many autistics do happily live in a [[suburb]] or large city. However, a metropolitan area can provide more opportunities for cultural and personal conflicts, requiring greater needs for adjustment.
'''Comorbidity:''' Accompanying impairments are also a common feature of autism. Some people with autism also have [[gastrointestinal]], [[immunological]] or [[neurological]] symptoms in addition to behavioral impairments. These associated complexes have also lead to the search for specific genetic connections and helped to focus on reasonable genetic implications.<ref name=NIHNews/>


In schools it is commonplace for autistics to be singled out by teachers and students as "unruly," though an autistic student may not understand why his or her actions are considered inappropriate, especially when the student has a logical explanation for his or her behavior.
Since genes provide the information for processes and structure at the level of the [[Cell (biology)|cell]] and its components during the growth and development of a human as well as maintenance during life, [[gene]] [[mutation]]s (altered versions) and deletions (complete absence of genetic material) and possibly extra copies of genes would mean that the causes of autism begin very early. If a mutated gene fails to perform properly, then [[Cell (biology)|cells]], [[proteins]], [[enzymes]] and other crucial aspects of normal function may be significantly altered and operate incorrectly. Deletions could mean the complete absence of a sequence of events due to missing proteins or cell components for example. These genetic alterations and deletions will simply bring about a changed structure or process which effects a great many other needed structures and processes.


The interests of autistic people and so-called "[[geek]]s" or "[[nerd]]s" can often overlap as autistic people can sometimes become preoccupied with certain subjects, much like anyone else. However, in practice many autistic people have difficulty with working in groups, which impairs them even in the most 'geeky' of situations. The connection of autism with so-called geek or nerd behavior has received attention in the popular press, but is still controversial within these groups.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.wired.com/wired/archive/9.12/aspergers_pr.html|title=Geeks and autism|accessdate=2007-01-26}}</ref>
===Deletions and mutations===
'''Gene deletion:''' Deleted genes may be an influence or cause of autism. <ref name=AJHG-Deletions/> By locating missing genetic material, specific DNA involved in non-autistic behavior (autism susceptibility alleles) may be identified. Another significant aspect of this research is that deleted genetic material indicates that autism may be established during meiosis (error-prone meiosis model) and this theoretically places the genesis of autism at the very beginning of life.


Speculation arises over famous people and celebrities who are now suspected, but unconfirmed, of having autism and Asperger's syndrome. They are rumored to have most symptoms of autism or autistic-spectrum disorder. Biographers, personal physicians and media journalists continually investigate these rumors, but some say that the claims are actually [[libel]]lous of their character as public figures, being singled out as "odd" or "nerdy" people.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.geocities.com/richardg_uk/famousac.html|title=List of famous people with autistic traits|accessdate=2007-01-26}}</ref>
One very important question in this line of research is whether or not specific gene deletions cause or are a consequence of autism-susceptibility loci located elsewhere in the chromosomes.


===Autistic adults===
'''Gene mutations:''' A mutation may mean a gene does not function at all or does not function in the normal way. Since genes direct how the body grows and develops, mutations, like deletions, will effect a person at the most basic levels.
Communication and social problems often cause difficulties in many areas of the autistic's life. A much smaller proportion of adult autistics marry or have children than the general population. Even when they do marry, some argue, it is more likely to end in divorce than the norm,<ref>{{cite web | title=Asperger syndrome and adults, Dr Isabelle Henault | url=http://www.betterhealth.vic.gov.au/bhcv2/bhcarticles.nsf/pages/Asperger_syndrome_and_adults?open | accessmonthday = June 12 | accessyear=2006 }}</ref> although further research should perhaps be made. Nevertheless, as more social groups form, progressively more diagnosed adults are forming relationships with others on the spectrum.


A small proportion of autistic adults, usually those with high-functioning autism or [[Asperger's syndrome]], are able to work successfully in mainstream jobs, although frequently far below their actual level of skills and qualification. Some have managed to gain self-employment.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.auties.org|title=Auties.org, a self employment site|accessdate=2007-01-26}}</ref>
Mutation and deletion effects have been delineated in numerous research publications.<ref>[http://www.jcb.org/cgi/content/full/167/5/945 The neuronal scaffold protein Shank3 mediates signaling and biological function of the receptor tyrosine kinase Ret in epithelial cells] G. Schuetz et al (2004) Journal of Cell Biology. Volume 167, Number 5, 945-952</ref><ref name=CAN>[http://www.cureautismnow.org/site/apps/nl/content2.asp?c=bhLOK2PILuF&b=1289189&ct=3422589  Researchers Find Rare Mutations in SHANK3 Gene Are Associated with Autism] Synopsis of information from Nature Genetics (January 2007). 4 citations. Cure Autism Now. Retrieved March 3, 2007</ref><ref>[http://synapse-web.org/lab/harris/Lecture10-11/sld069.htm Neuroligins] Kristen Harris (2001) Cell adhesion at synapses Synapse Web, Laboratory of Synapse Structure and Function. Human Brain Project. National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of Drug Abuse</ref><ref> [http://stke.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/abstract/2005/265/tw8 Neuroligins Organize Excitatory and Inhibitory Postsynaptic Membrane] E. R. Graf et al, (2004). Cell Issue 119, pages 1013-1026</ref><ref>[http://www.autismvox.com/neuroligin-mutations-as-a-cause-of-autism/ Neuroligin Mutations as a Cause of Autism] K. Chew Autism Vox Retrieved March 3, 2007</ref>  


Others are employed in sheltered workshops under the supervision of managers trained in working with persons with disabilities. A nurturing environment at home, at school, and later in job training and at work, helps autistic people continue to learn and to develop throughout their lives.
'''Gene interaction:''' Interactions between genetic material may also complicate the causes leading to multiple genetic origins of autism. <ref name=CAN/><ref name=Phelan/> In a cascade like effect, when a gene loci is altered or omitted, others are effected due to change in interaction between genes and/or their functions.


It is often said that the [[Internet]], since it is almost devoid of the non-verbal cues that autistics find so hard to interact with, has given some autistic individuals an environment in which they can, and do, communicate and form online communities. The internet has also provided the option of occupations such as, [[Telecommuting|teleworking]] and independent consulting, which, in general, do not require much human interaction offline.
'''Correlated characteristics:''' Effects that accompany deletions and mutations include global developmental delay, mild to severe delay of speech, social communication disorders and cognitive abilities, autistic like behaviour, high tolerance of pain, and repetitive mannerisms (e.g. chewing or mouthing). <ref name=Phelan>[http://www.orpha.net/data/patho/GB/uk-22q13.pdf Deletion 22q13 Syndrome] M.C Phelan (2003) Orphanet.com</ref>


Under the public law, in the [[United States]], the public schools' responsibility for providing services ends when the autistic person is 21 years of age. The autistic person and their family are then faced with the challenge of finding living arrangements and employment to match their particular needs, as well as the programs and facilities that can provide support services to achieve these goals.
'''Practical applications for research:''' Though not present in all individuals with autism, these mutations and deletions hold potential to point the way to other genetic factors involved in spectrum disorders. <ref>December 2006; doi:10.1038/ng1933, Nature Genetics)</ref> This type of research may lead to the development of a test that would confirm the autism diagnosis in children exhibiting symptoms and identify families who carry genetic defects that could be inherited by their children. The research also advances basic understanding in the genetic architecture of the genome of autistic individuals and helps in focusing future research.


Many parents of autistic children also face financial difficulties as they must often pay for essential support and therapeutic services. Furthermore, autism is often linked to [[poverty]] because autistics who might qualify for [[welfare (financial aid)|financial assistance]] in one country are not eligible in another, because some nations do not recognize autism as a disability.
===Environmental components===
Another important aspect of research in ASDs is environmental effects and the incidence of autism. Environmental factors such as mercury and radiation have been proposed as possible causes of ASD. According to this theoretical explanation, during the lifetime of a person, gene mutations and deletions may be environmentally triggered or exacerbated. Conversely, it may also be that environment will not be a factor and nothing will change the autism characteristics: the nature of the sporadic CNVs does not imply the presence of an environmental agent. <ref name=Reuters-glitches/>


===Terminology===
'''Mercury'''
When referring to someone who is diagnosed with autism, the term "autistic" is often used. Alternatively, many prefer to use the [[person-first terminology]] "person with autism" or "person who experiences autism." However, it has been noted that members of the [[autistic community]] generally prefer "autistic person" for reasons that are fairly controversial.<ref> [http://www.autismsocietyofwa.org/DiaryJuly-Aug05.pdf The Diary, July-August 2005], publication of the Autism Society of Washington, page 4, accessed [[4 February]] [[2007]]  Autistic adults at the Autism Society of America 2005 Conference felt that the term "individuals with autism" separates their autism from who they are. In other words, they believe their autism is part of who they are and want to be called "autistic adults.</ref> This article uses both terminologies.


===Autistic savants===
Current recommandations about fish intake in children and pregnant women are determined in function of the risk of mercury exposure, despite the fact that fish, in itself, provides many nutrients required for proper brain function. Mercury, in the form of methylmercury (its organic form, as opposed to pure mercury), at levels encountered in foetuses of mothers eating fish regularly, is associated with mental impairments consistent with autism. Methylmercury impairs the regulation of the most important excitatory neurotransmitter, glutamate, by impairing the ability of [[glial cells]] to "wash it" from the space between neurons, thus causing a form of neuronal degeneration called ''[[excitotoxicity]]''.<ref name="pmid10812205">{{cite journal |author=Aschner M, Yao CP, Allen JW, Tan KH |title=Methylmercury alters glutamate transport in astrocytes |journal=Neurochem. Int. |volume=37 |issue=2-3 |pages=199–206 |year=2000 |pmid=10812205 |doi=}}</ref>
{{Main|Autistic savant}}
In addition, researchers from the University of Texas correlated the amount of mercury released in different school districts of Texas to the number of cases of autism: each 1000 lb of environmentally released mercury appeared to increase by 43 % the rates of autism.<ref name="pmid16338635">{{cite journal |author=Palmer RF, Blanchard S, Stein Z, Mandell D, Miller C |title=Environmental mercury release, special education rates, and autism disorder: an ecological study of Texas |journal=Health & place |volume=12 |issue=2 |pages=203–9 |year=2006 |pmid=16338635 |doi=10.1016/j.healthplace.2004.11.005 |issn=}}</ref>
The autistic savant phenomenon is sometimes seen in autistic people. Estimates of the prevalence of this phenomenon range between 1% and 10%.<ref>{{cite web | title= Savant prevalence | url=http://wisconsinmedicalsociety.org/savant/synopsis_article.cfm | accessmonthday = June 23 | accessyear=2006 }}</ref> The term is used to describe a person who is autistic and has extreme talent in a certain area of study. Although there is a common association between savants and autism (an association made especially popular by the 1988 film ''[[Rain Man]]''), most autistic people are not [[savant]]s and savantism is not unique to autistic people, though there does seem to be some relation.<ref>Heaton, P. & Wallace, G.L. (2004). Annotation: The savant syndrome. ''Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(5),'' 899-911.</ref> [[Mental calculator]]s and fast [[computer programming]] skills are the most common form. A well known example is [[Daniel Tammet]], the subject of the [[documentary film]] ''[[The Brain Man]]''<ref>{{cite web|title=Guardian "Brain Man" article|url=http://www.guardian.co.uk/weekend/story/0,3605,1409903,00.html| accessmonthday = July 30 | accessyear=2005 }}</ref> ([[Kim Peek]], one of the inspirations for [[Dustin Hoffman]]'s character in the film ''Rain Man'', is not autistic). ''Bright Splinters of the Mind'' by Beate Hermelin is a book that explores this issue further.


==Other pervasive developmental disorders==
==Other pervasive developmental disorders==
Autism and Asperger's syndrome are just two of the five pervasive developmental disorders (PDDs). The three other pervasive developmental disorders are [[Rett syndrome]], [[Childhood disintegrative disorder]], and [[PDD not otherwise specified|Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified]]. Some of these are related to autism, while some of them are entirely separate conditions.
Autism and Asperger's syndrome are two of the five pervasive developmental disorders (PDDs); the three others are [[Rett syndrome]], [[Childhood disintegrative disorder]], and [[PDD not otherwise specified|Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified]]. Some of these are related to autism, while some of them are entirely separate conditions.
 
 


==Notes==
==Notes==
Line 301: Line 223:
<div class="references-small" style="-moz-column-count:2; column-count:2;">
<div class="references-small" style="-moz-column-count:2; column-count:2;">
<references/>
<references/>
</div>
</div>[[Category:Suggestion Bot Tag]]
 
[[Category:Health Sciences Workgroup]]

Latest revision as of 16:01, 14 July 2024

This article is developing and not approved.
Main Article
Discussion
Related Articles  [?]
Bibliography  [?]
External Links  [?]
Citable Version  [?]
Video [?]
 
This editable Main Article is under development and subject to a disclaimer.
Autism
ICD-10 F84.0-F84.1
ICD-9 ICD9 299
OMIM 209850
MedlinePlus 001526

Autism (pronounced IPA /'ɔtizm/) is classified by the World Health Organization and American Psychological Association as a developmental disability that results from a disorder of the human central nervous system. It is diagnosed using specific criteria for impairments to social interaction, communication, interests, imagination and activities.[1]

Autism manifests before the age of three years, according to the World Health Organization's International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10)[2]. Autistic children are marked by delays in their "social interaction, language as used in social communication, or symbolic or imaginative play" (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders).[3]

Autism and the other four pervasive developmental disorders (PDD) are considered to be neurodevelopmental disorders. They are diagnosed on the basis of a triad of behavioral impairments or dysfunctions: 1. impaired social interaction, 2. impaired communication and 3. restricted and repetitive interests and activities.[4]

From a physiological standpoint, autism is often less than obvious, in that outward appearance may not indicate a disorder. Diagnosis typically comes from a complete patient history and physical and neurological evaluation.

The incidence of diagnosed autism has increased since the 1990s.[5] Reasons offered for this phenomenon include better diagnosis, wider public awareness of the condition, regional variations in diagnostic criteria, or an actual increase in the occurrence of ASD (autism spectrum disorders). The United States Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) estimate the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders to be about one in every 150 children.[6][7] In 2005, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) stated the "best conservative estimate" as 1 in 1000.[8] In 2006, NIMH estimated that the incidence was 2-6 in every 1000[9]

There are numerous theories as to the specific causes of autism, but they are as yet unproven (see section on "Causes" below). Proposed factors include genetic influence, anatomical variations (e.g. head circumference), abnormal blood vessel function and oxidative stress. Their significance as well as implications for treatment remain speculative.

Terminology

When referring to someone who is diagnosed with autism, the term "autistic" is often used. Alternatively, many prefer to use the person-first terminology, i.e. "person with autism" or "person who experiences autism." However, it has been noted that members of the autistic community generally prefer "autistic person" for reasons that are fairly controversial.[10] This article uses both terminologies.

History

The word "autism" was first used in the English language by Swiss psychiatrist Eugene Bleuler in a 1912 issue of the American Journal of Insanity. It comes from the Greek word for "self," αυτος (autos).[11] Autism was actually confused with schizophrenia during the early stages of observation.[12] Bleuler used the term to describe schizophrenics' seeming difficulty in connecting with other people.[13]

However, the medical classification of autism as a separate disorder or disease did not occur until 1943 when psychiatrist Dr. Leo Kanner of the Johns Hopkins Hospital in Baltimore, Maryland reported on 11 child patients with striking behavioral similarities and introduced the label "early infantile autism."[14] He suggested the term "autism" to describe the fact that the children seemed to lack interest in other people, emphasizing "autistic aloneness" and "insistence on sameness". Kanner's first paper on the subject was published in the journal The Nervous Child (no longer in publication),[15] and almost every characteristic he originally described is still regarded as typical of the autistic spectrum of disorders diagnostic triad of behavioral impairments or dysfunctions: 1. impaired social interaction, 2. impaired communication and 3. restricted and repetitive interests and activities.[16].[17]

Leo Kanner's contemporary in Austria, Dr. Hans Asperger, made similar observations, although his name has since become attached to a different form of autism known as Asperger's Syndrome. Widespread recognition of Asperger's work was delayed by World War II in Germany. His seminal paper was not actually translated into English for almost 50 years and the majority of his work was not widely read until 1997.[18]

Autism and Asperger's Syndrome are today listed in the DSM-IV-TR as two of the five pervasive developmental disorders (PDD), which also include Childhood disintegrative disorder,[19][20] Rett syndrome[21] and Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (or atypical autism). Health care providers also refer to autism spectrum disorders (ASD) which includes only three of those listed in PDD: Autistic disorder, Asperger syndrome, Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified.[22] All of these conditions are characterized by varying degrees of deficiencies in communication skil]s and social interactions, along with restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of human behavior.

Characteristics

On the surface, individuals who have autism are physically indistinguishable from those without. Some studies show that autistic children tend to have larger head circumferences[23][24] but the significance in the disorder is unclear. Sometimes autism co-occurs with other disorders, and in those cases outward differences may be apparent.

Individuals diagnosed with autism can vary greatly in skills and behaviors, and their response to sensory input shows marked differences in a number of ways from that of other people. Certain stimuli, such as sounds, lights, and touch, will often affect someone with autism differently than someone without, and the degree to which the sensory system is affected can vary greatly from one individual to another.[25]

Key behaviors

Autistic children may display unusual behaviors or fail to display expected behaviors. Normal behaviors may develop at the appropriate age and then disappear or, conversely, are delayed and develop quite some time after normal occurrence. In assessing developmental delays, different physicians may not always arrive at the same conclusions. Much of this difference between diagnosis is due to the disputed criteria for autism.[26] Disagreement on deciding how a child should normally behave also makes it difficult to construct objective tests of child behavior.

Essentially, the diagnosis of autism must meet specific criteria but there are also many characteristics that are idiosyncratic. Thus, autism is not a "one size fits all" label. As a result, the spectrum disorder encompasses a very wide range of behaviors and symptoms.

Some behaviors cited by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (listed below) may simply mean a normal delay in one or more areas of development, while others are more typical of ASDs—Autistic Spectrum Disorders.[22]

The list below is not all-inclusive, and generally applies to children and not adults. Furthermore, while some of these behaviors might be seen in a person with autism, others may be absent.[22]

Noted behaviours

  • does not respond to his/her name
  • cannot explain what he/she wants
  • language skills are slow to develop or speech is delayed
  • does not follow directions
  • will fuss if didn't get what wanted
  • at times, the child seems to be deaf
  • seems to hear sometimes, but not other times
  • doesn't point or wave "bye-bye"
  • doesn't understand the concept of pointing; will look at the hand pointing rather than the object being pointed at
  • used to say a few words or babble, but now he/she doesn't
  • throws intense or violent tantrums
  • has odd movement patterns
  • likes to spin around in a circle
  • likes being in a well known place
  • hands often cover ears
  • is overly active, uncooperative, or resistant
  • doesn't know how to play with toys
  • doesn't smile when smiled at
  • has poor eye contact
  • gets "stuck" doing the same things over and over and can't move on to other things
  • seems to prefer to play alone
  • gets things for him/herself only
  • is very independent for his/her age
  • does things "early" compared to other children
  • seems to be in his/her "own world."
  • seems to tune people out
  • is not interested in other children
  • dislikes playing pretend
  • walks on his/her toes
  • shows unusual attachments to toys, objects, or schedules (i.e., always holding a string or having to put socks on before pants)
  • spends a lot of time stacking objects, lining things up or putting things in a certain order
  • unconcerned about - or completely oblivious to - dangers around him/her (e.g., standing in the middle of the street without worrying about getting hit by a car)

Autism and blindness

The characteristics of a person with both an Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and a severe visual impairment (VI) may vary from a person with just ASD or just VI.[27] Historically, many behaviors of blind children were seen as "autistic-like" but were attributed to their blindness rather than pursuing possibilities of autism.[28]

Developmental trajectories of children with ASD-VI are often very similar as those followed by children with typical autism, but the child with ASD-VI will have particularly unusual responses to sensory information. The person may be overly sensitive to touch or sound, or be less responsive to pain. Typically, touch, smell, and sound are affected the most dramatically.[29]

Repetitive behaviors

Although people with autism usually appear physically normal, unusual repetitive motions, known as self-stimulation or "stimming," may set them apart. These behaviors might be extreme or subtle. Some children and older individuals spend a lot of time repeatedly flapping their arms or wiggling their toes, others suddenly freeze in position. Some spend hours arranging objects in a certain way rather than engaging in pretend play as a typical child might, and becoming agitated if they are re-arranged or moved. Repetitive behaviors can also extend into the spoken word; perseveration of a single word or phrase can also become a part of the child's daily routine. Some may repeat words from movies and watch certain bits over and over again.[30][31] Autistic children may demand consistency in their environment. A slight change in the timing, format or route of a routine or trip can be extremely disturbing to them. Autistics sometimes have persistent, intense preoccupations. For example, the child might be obsessed with learning all about computers, television programs, lighthouses or virtually any other topic.[22]


Types of autism

Autism presents in a wide degree, from those who are socially dysfunctional and apparently mentally disabled to those whose symptoms are mild or remedied enough to appear unexceptional ("normal") to others. Although this is controversial, autistic individuals are often divided into those with an IQ less than 80 (referred to as having "low-functioning autism" or LFA), and those with an IQ over 80 (referred to as having "high-functioning autism" (HFA).[32][33][34]) Low and high functioning are more generally applied to how well an individual can accomplish activities of daily living, rather than to IQ. The terms low and high functioning are controversial and not all autistics accept these labels. Additionally, a review of the literature in 2005 questioned the validity of IQ testing of autistic people, noting the frequency of poor methodology in numerous studies that assumed or failed to demonstrate impaired cognitive functioning.[35][36]

This discrepancy can lead to confusion among service providers who equate IQ with functioning and may refuse to serve high-IQ autistic people who are severely compromised in their ability to perform daily living tasks, or may fail to recognize the intellectual potential of many autistic people who are considered LFA. For example, some professionals refuse to recognize autistics who can speak or write as being autistic at all, because they still think of autism as a communication disorder so severe that no speech or writing is possible.

As a consequence, many "high-functioning" autistic persons, and autistic people with relatively high IQ, are under-diagnosed, thus making the claim that "autism implies retardation" self-fulfilling. The number of people diagnosed with LFA is not rising quite as sharply as HFA, indicating that at least part of the explanation for the apparent rise is probably better diagnostics. Many also think that ASD's are being over diagnosed: (1) because the growth in the number and complexity of symptoms associated with autism has increased the chances professionals will erroneously diagnose autism and (2) because the growth in services and therapies for autism has increased the number who falsely qualify for those often free services and therapies.

Asperger's Syndrome and Classic Autism Disorder

For more information, see: Asperger syndrome.

In the current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR), the most significant difference between Autistic Disorder and Asperger's syndrome is that a diagnosis of the former includes the observation of "delays or abnormal functioning in at least one of the following areas, with onset prior to age 3 years: (1) social interaction, (2) language as used in social communication, or (3) symbolic or imaginative play",[37] while a diagnosis of Asperger's syndrome observes "no clinically significant delay" in the latter two of these areas.[38]

While the DSM-IV does not include level of intellectual functioning in the diagnosis, the fact that those with Asperger's syndrome tend to perform better than those with classic autism has produced a popular conception that Asperger's syndrome is synonymous with "higher-functioning autism", or that it is a lesser disorder than autism.

The extent to which someone with higher functioning autism or Asperger's syndrome may excel is theoretically quite high. For example, Henry Cavendish, one of history's foremost scientists, may have been autistic. George Wilson, a notable chemist and physician, wrote a book about Cavendish entitled, "The Life of the Honourable Henry Cavendish", published in 1851. From Wilson's detailed description it seems that while Cavendish may have exhibited many classic signs of autism, he nevertheless had an extraordinary mind.[39]

Autism as a spectrum disorder

Another view of these disorders is that they are on a continuum known as autistic spectrum disorders. Autism spectrum disorder is an increasingly popular term that refers to a broad definition of autism including the classic form of the disorder as well as closely related conditions such as PDD-NOS (Pervasive Developmental Disorders-Not Otherwise Specified) and Asperger's syndrome. Although the classic form of autism can be easily distinguished from other forms of autism spectrum disorder, the terms are often used interchangeably.

A related continuum, Sensory Integration Dysfunction, involves how well humans integrate the information they receive from their senses. Autism, Asperger's syndrome, and Sensory Integration Dysfunction are all closely related and overlap.

Some people believe that there might be two manifestations of classical autism, regressive autism and early infantile autism. Early infantile autism is present at birth while regressive autism begins before the age of 3 and often around 18 months. Although this causes some controversy over when the neurological differences involved in autism truly begin, some speculate that an environmental influence or toxin triggers the disorder. This triggering could occur during gestation due to a toxin that enters the mother's body and is transferred to the fetus. The triggering could also occur after birth during the crucial early nervous system development of the child.

A paper published in 2006 concerning the behavioral, cognitive, and genetic bases of autism argues that autism should perhaps not be seen as a single disorder, but rather as a set of distinct symptoms (social difficulties, communicative difficulties and repetitive behaviors) that have their own distinct causes.[40] An implication of this would be that a search for a "cure" for autism is unlikely to succeed if it is not examined as separate, albeit overlapping and commonly co-occurring, disorders.

Epidemiology

Gender differences

The ratio of incidence between males and females (as demonstrated in the scientific literature) is ambiguous. Studies have found much higher prevalence in males at the high-functioning end of the spectrum, while the ratios appear to be closer to 1:1 at the low-functioning end.[41] In addition, a study published in 2006 suggested that males over 40 are more likely than younger males to parent a child with autism, and that the ratio of autism incidence in males and females is closer to 1:1 with older fathers.[42][43]

Reported increase with time

There was a worldwide increase in reported cases of autism over the decade leading up to 2006. There are several theories about this apparent sudden increase.

Many epidemiologists argue that the rise in the incidence of autism in the United States is largely attributable to a broadening of the diagnostic concept, earlier diagnosis (thereby accounting for those cases which are actually diagnosed as early as one year of age)[44] reclassifications, public awareness, and the incentive to receive federally mandated services.[45] However, some authors indicate that the existence of an as yet unidentified contributing environmental risk factor cannot be ruled out.[46] A widely-cited pilot study conducted in California by the UC Davis M.I.N.D. Institute (17 October, 2002), reported that the increase in autism in California is real, even after accounting for changes to diagnostic criteria.[47]

The question of whether the rise in incidence is real or an artifact of improved diagnosis and a broader concept of autism remains controversial. Dr. Chris Johnson, a professor of pediatrics at the University of Texas Health Sciences Center at San Antonio and co-chair of the American Academy of Pediatrics Autism Expert Panel, sums up the state of the issue by saying, "There is a chance we're seeing a true rise, but right now I do not think anybody can answer that question for sure." [48]

Treatment

U.S. President George W. Bush signing a piece of legislation aimed to combat autism

The efficacy of treating autism is disputed. There is a broad array of proposed autism therapies with various goals, e.g. improving health and well-being, emotional problems, difficulties with communication and learning, and sensory problems for people with autism, but the effectiveness of these approaches is not clear.

Antipsychotics are used to manage disruptive and violent behavior, such as irritability, tantrums and violence. Risperidone (trade name Risperdal) is the only anti-psychotic agent currently approved by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration to treat children and adolescents with autism.[49] Risperidone benefited patients in a randomized controlled trial.[50]

Physiology and neurology

Autism appears to involve a greater amount of the brain than previously thought.[51] A study of 112 children (56 with autism and 56 without), published in the Journal of Child Neuropsychology, found those with autism to have more problems with complex tasks, such as tying their shoelaces or writing, which suggests that many areas of the brain are involved.[52] Children with autism performed simple tasks as well as or better than those without. In tests of visual and spatial skills, autistic children did well at finding small objects in complex pictures (e.g., finding the character Waldo in "Where's Waldo" pictures). However, they found it difficult to tell the difference between similar-looking people. Children with autism tended to do well in spelling and grammar, but found it much more difficult to understand complex speech, such as idioms or similes when the meaning of the phrase is figurative. They would, for example, not understand that "He kicked the bucket" meant someone had died, or were likely to actually hop if told to "hop to it".

The research from this perspective has a number of implications:

  • Autism is more than likely a global disorder which affects how the brain processes the information it receives, while complex information tends to make this more readily apparent.
  • Neurological ‘wiring’ in people with autism manifest abnormalities in the areas of the brain that communicate with each other.
  • Observed abnormalities provide a reasonable explanation for why children with autism have problems with complex tasks which require multiple areas of the brain to work together; autistic people tend to do better in tasks that only require one region of the brain.
  • The causes of autism are possibly more pervasive than previously believed; for example, more areas of the brain are affected than just those involving social interaction, communication, interests, and imagination.
  • Autism may not be primarily a disorder of social interaction; research must now take into account non-social aspects.

A possible explanation for the characteristics of the syndrome is a variation in the way the brain itself reacts to sensory input and how parts of the brain then handle the information. An electroencephalographic (EEG) study of 36 adults (half of whom had autism) at Washington University in St. Louis found that adults with autism show differences in the manner in which neural activity is coordinated. The implication seems to be that there is poor internal communication between different areas of the brain. (Electroencephalographs, or EEGs, measure the activity of brain cells.)

The Wash. U. study indicated that there were abnormal patterns in the way the brain cells were connected in the temporal lobe of the brain. (The temporal lobe deals with language.) These abnormal patterns would seem to indicate inefficient and inconsistent communication inside the brain of autistic people.[53]

Studies in neuropathology indicate abnormalities in the amygdala, hippocampus, septum, mamillary bodies, limbic system,and the cerebellum. [54][55]

  • Autistic brains are slightly larger and heavier and a larger than normal head circumference is commonly noted.
  • In the limbic system, there is an excess of cells and they are too small. The neurons themselves appear to be underdeveloped. Dendritic trees which provide the basis for connections between neurons are truncated (i.e. shortened).
  • In the cerebellum, purkinje cells are widely affected. The anatomic differences correlate to the curtailment of development earlier than 30 weeks gestation. In other words, the development of the cells appears to have stopped at some time before the 30th week in utero
  • An enlarged third ventricle of the brain appears to accompany autism in those who are non-mentally retarded, but the reasons for this and its effects are still unknown.

Research has not yet established exactly what is specific to autism and what may be seen in other disorders however.[54]

Individuals with autism are also far more likely to develop epilepsy than would otherwise be expected (estimated 10-30% incidence). [56]

Mirror neurons

A theory featuring mirror neurons[57][58] states that autism may involve a dysfunction of specialized neurons in the brain that should activate when observing other people. In typically-developing people, these mirror neurons are thought to perhaps play a major part in social learning and general comprehension of the actions of others.

Causes

The causes and etiology of autism are uncertain. Possible genetic and environmental causes are a common focus in published literature. Since autistic individuals are all somewhat different from one another, multiple "causes" have been proposed that interact with each other in subtle and complex ways, and thus give slightly differing outcomes in each individual.

Research claims also link autism with abnormal blood vessel function, and oxidative stress. [59]

Genetic component

Extent of genetic origins of autism spectrum disorders: Genetic influence comprises a significant aspect of research in the causes of autism.[60]

More than one hundred different genes have been implicated in the causes of autism spectrum disorders (ASD). One type of genetic origin are “glitches” or small changes in DNA that are not genetic mutations, per se, but copy number variations (CNVs) which are extra copies or missing stretches of DNA. (In one case study a child with Asperger’s Syndrome was found to be missing a chain of 27 genes.) The extent of the possible genetic origins indicate that the abilities in social interaction and behavior, social and physical composure and impulse control are scattered throughout the human genome, implying that small genetic alterations, copies or deletions anywhere can have broad effects[61]

A large database showing theoretical links between autism and genetic loci summarises research indicating that the genetic influence may extend to every human chromosome.[62] It has been observed in one twin-study in Britain that there was about a 60% concordance rate for autism in monozygotic (identical) twins,[63] while dizygotic (non-identical) twins and other siblings comparatively exhibited about 4% concordance rates.[64] Some research posits that the chances that an identical twin of an autistic person will also be autistic are 85-90%.[65] The increased probabilities of siblings having autism has been calculated at about 35-fold more than normal.[66]

Generational links, or familial heritability may, however, be less prevalent than some research to date implies. Spontaneous changes in DNA are much more common than is usual in other diseases with a genetic component. The sporadic form of the disease accounts for about 90% of affected individuals. [61]

Comorbidity: Accompanying impairments are also a common feature of autism. Some people with autism also have gastrointestinal, immunological or neurological symptoms in addition to behavioral impairments. These associated complexes have also lead to the search for specific genetic connections and helped to focus on reasonable genetic implications.[66]

Since genes provide the information for processes and structure at the level of the cell and its components during the growth and development of a human as well as maintenance during life, gene mutations (altered versions) and deletions (complete absence of genetic material) and possibly extra copies of genes would mean that the causes of autism begin very early. If a mutated gene fails to perform properly, then cells, proteins, enzymes and other crucial aspects of normal function may be significantly altered and operate incorrectly. Deletions could mean the complete absence of a sequence of events due to missing proteins or cell components for example. These genetic alterations and deletions will simply bring about a changed structure or process which effects a great many other needed structures and processes.

Deletions and mutations

Gene deletion: Deleted genes may be an influence or cause of autism. [60] By locating missing genetic material, specific DNA involved in non-autistic behavior (autism susceptibility alleles) may be identified. Another significant aspect of this research is that deleted genetic material indicates that autism may be established during meiosis (error-prone meiosis model) and this theoretically places the genesis of autism at the very beginning of life.

One very important question in this line of research is whether or not specific gene deletions cause or are a consequence of autism-susceptibility loci located elsewhere in the chromosomes.

Gene mutations: A mutation may mean a gene does not function at all or does not function in the normal way. Since genes direct how the body grows and develops, mutations, like deletions, will effect a person at the most basic levels.

Mutation and deletion effects have been delineated in numerous research publications.[67][68][69][70][71]

Gene interaction: Interactions between genetic material may also complicate the causes leading to multiple genetic origins of autism. [68][72] In a cascade like effect, when a gene loci is altered or omitted, others are effected due to change in interaction between genes and/or their functions.

Correlated characteristics: Effects that accompany deletions and mutations include global developmental delay, mild to severe delay of speech, social communication disorders and cognitive abilities, autistic like behaviour, high tolerance of pain, and repetitive mannerisms (e.g. chewing or mouthing). [72]

Practical applications for research: Though not present in all individuals with autism, these mutations and deletions hold potential to point the way to other genetic factors involved in spectrum disorders. [73] This type of research may lead to the development of a test that would confirm the autism diagnosis in children exhibiting symptoms and identify families who carry genetic defects that could be inherited by their children. The research also advances basic understanding in the genetic architecture of the genome of autistic individuals and helps in focusing future research.

Environmental components

Another important aspect of research in ASDs is environmental effects and the incidence of autism. Environmental factors such as mercury and radiation have been proposed as possible causes of ASD. According to this theoretical explanation, during the lifetime of a person, gene mutations and deletions may be environmentally triggered or exacerbated. Conversely, it may also be that environment will not be a factor and nothing will change the autism characteristics: the nature of the sporadic CNVs does not imply the presence of an environmental agent. [61]

Mercury

Current recommandations about fish intake in children and pregnant women are determined in function of the risk of mercury exposure, despite the fact that fish, in itself, provides many nutrients required for proper brain function. Mercury, in the form of methylmercury (its organic form, as opposed to pure mercury), at levels encountered in foetuses of mothers eating fish regularly, is associated with mental impairments consistent with autism. Methylmercury impairs the regulation of the most important excitatory neurotransmitter, glutamate, by impairing the ability of glial cells to "wash it" from the space between neurons, thus causing a form of neuronal degeneration called excitotoxicity.[74] In addition, researchers from the University of Texas correlated the amount of mercury released in different school districts of Texas to the number of cases of autism: each 1000 lb of environmentally released mercury appeared to increase by 43 % the rates of autism.[75]

Other pervasive developmental disorders

Autism and Asperger's syndrome are two of the five pervasive developmental disorders (PDDs); the three others are Rett syndrome, Childhood disintegrative disorder, and Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified. Some of these are related to autism, while some of them are entirely separate conditions.

Notes

  1. Diagnostic Criteria for Autistic Disorder Indiana Resource Center for Autism, Indiana Institute on Disability and Community, Indiana University. Retrieved Feb. 27, 2007
  2. [1] ICD version 2006, World Health Organisation Codes for Pervasive developmental disorders, F84.0-F84.9, retrieved 20 January, 2007
  3. more commonly known as the DSM-IV-TR, i.e. the fourth edition with text revision. Relevant DSM codes are: 307.9 Communication Disorder Not Otherwise Specified, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 299.00 Autistic Disorder, 299.80 Rett's Disorder, 299.10 Childhood Disintegrative Disorder, 299.80 Asperger's Disorder, 299.80 Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (Including Atypical Autism), Attention-Deficit and Disruptive Behavior Disorders, Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
  4. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 1994)
  5. "Autism 'more common than thought'" BBC News (2006) July 13
  6. ABC News Autism More Common Than Thought in U.S.: Survey>. Retrieved on 2007-01-25.
  7. U.S. Says Autism Rate 1 in 150 Mike Stobbe. Forbes.com 2 Feb. 2007. Retrieved Feb 9, 2007
  8. NIH Autism Overview 2005. Retrieved on 2006-02-05.
  9. New NIMH Research Program Launches Autism Trials (page 4) National Institute of Mental Health. September 7, 2006
  10. The Diary, July-August 2005, publication of the Autism Society of Washington, page 4, accessed 4 February 2007 Autistic adults at the Autism Society of America 2005 Conference felt that the term "individuals with autism" separates their autism from who they are. In other words, they believe their autism is part of who they are and want to be called "autistic adults.
  11. Autism Through Ages Baffles Science Robert Williams Jr., Pediatric Services
  12. Interview with Lorna Jean King, OTR, FAOTA. Retrieved on 2007-01-25.
  13. "Epistemological aspects of Eugen Bleuler's conception of schizophrenia in 1911." Stotz-Ingenlath G., Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy. 2000;3(2):153-9. ISSN:1386-7423
  14. "Autism Spectrum Disorders (Pervasive Developmental Disorders)" National Institute of Mental Health
  15. Kanner, L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 2, 217–250 (1943)
  16. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 1994)
  17. Time to give up on a single explanation for autism Francesca Happé, Angelica Ronald & Robert Plomin (2006) Nature Neuroscience - 9, 1218 - 1220. Published online: 26 September 2006
  18. "What Is Asperger Syndrome?" Barbara L. Kirby, Online Asperger Syndrome Information and Support
  19. *Frombonne E. (2002). "Prevalence of childhood disintegrative disorder". Autism 6 (2): 149-157.
  20. Volkmar RM and Rutter M. (1995). "Childhood disintegrative disorder: Results of the DSM-IV autism field trial". Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 34: 1092-1095.
  21. Rett syndrome (NIH Publication No. 01-4960). Rett syndrome. Rockville, MD: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (2001). Retrieved on July 30, 2005.
  22. 22.0 22.1 22.2 22.3 "Autism Overview: What we know" Filipek et al. (1999). Screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(6): 439-484. Cited in NICHD publications reference. Retrieved 26 Jan. 2007
  23. Center for Autism Research article on head circumference Retrieved November 13, 2006
  24. Lainhart JE, Bigler ED, Bocian M, Coon H, Dinh E, Dawson G, Deutsch CK, Dunn M, Estes A, Tager-Flusberg H, Folstein S, Hepburn S, Hyman S, McMahon W, Minshew N, Munson J, Osann K, Ozonoff S, Rodier P, Rogers S, Sigman M, Spence MA, Stodgell CJ, Volkmar F. 2006. >on-line abstract
  25. "Study Provides Evidence That Autism Affects Functioning of Entire Brain-Previous View Held Autism Limited to Communication, Social Behavior, and Reasoning" National Institutes of Health, (Aug, 16, 2006)
  26. The Epidemiology of Asperger Syndrome: A Total Population Study Stephan Ehlers and Christopher Gillberg (1993) The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Vol. 34, No. 8, pp. 1327-1350, November. Reprinted with Permission by Cambridge University Press. This seminal article discusses the nature of variations in diagnostic criteria in Asperger's and Autism
  27. Gense, Marilyn H., and D. Jay Gense. "Autism Spectrum Disorders in Learners with Blindness/Vision Impairments." RE: View (1994). 5 May 2006 <http://focusfamilies.org/focus/docs/blindnessandautism.pdf PDF>
  28. Examples of Symptom Presentation in Blind/Autistic Children in Contrast to Blind/Non-Autistic Children a la DSM-IV Criter. Retrieved on 2007-01-25. T.Pawletko & L. Rocissano (2000) Texas School for Blind and Visually Impaired
  29. Info on autism. Retrieved on 2007-01-25.
  30. Crosland, K.A.; et al. (2001-05-25). Prevalence of stereotypy among children diagnosed with autism at a tertiary referral clinic (pdf). Presented at the Association for Behavioral Analysis annual conference. Retrieved on 2006-07-01.
  31. Edelson, Stephen M. (1995). Stereotypic (Self-Stimulatory) Behavior (Stimming). Retrieved on 2006-07-01.
  32. Autism Spectrum Disorders (Pervasive Developmental Disorders) National Institute of Mental Health (2004) Retrieved 24 March, 2007
  33. Facts on autism. Retrieved on 2007-01-25.
  34. A search of the National Institute of Health database [2] March 24, 2007 showed 12 papers or studies that employed the distinction "high/higher functioning autism". A similar search of the PubMed database [3] elicited 486 articles and entries employing the term "high-functioning autism" published between June, 1975 and April, 2007
  35. Edelson, M.G. (2006). Are the majority of children with autism mentally retarded?: A systematic evaluation of the data. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 21, 66-83. Reprint online at http://www.willamette.edu/dept/comm/reprint/edelson/, retrieved 15 April 2007
  36. "Many times, if the researchers had a child they couldn't test, they just assumed he or she was retarded and assigned a low IQ score." Professor challenges autism assumption. The Oregonian, November 25, 2006.[4]. Retrieved 2007-02-25.
  37. BehaveNet autism description. Retrieved on July 30, 2005.
  38. BehaveNet aspergers description. Retrieved on July 30, 2005.
  39. "Henry Cavendish: An early case of Asperger's syndrome?" Oliver Sachs (Albert Einstein College of Medicine) Neurological Foundation of New Zealand (Reprinted with permission from the American Neurological Association)
  40. Happe F, Ronald A, Plomin R. Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nat Neurosci. 2006 October;9(10):1218-20.[5]
  41. Volkmar, F.R., Szatmari, P. & Sparro, S.S. (1993). Sex differences in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(4), 579-591.
  42. Reichenberg, Abraham; et al. (September 2006). "Advancing Paternal Age and Autism" (PDF). Archives of General Psychiatry 63 (9): 1026-1032.
  43. Vince, Gaia. Older dads boost risk of autistic children, NewScientist.com, September 2006. Layman's synopsis of the A. Reichenberg article
  44. Infants at Risk of Autism: A Longitudinal Study STAART Network Centers: University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), National Institute of Mental Health
  45. Jick, H; JA Kaye (December 2003). "Epidemiology and causes of autism". Pharmacotherapy 23 (12): 1524-30.
  46. Rutter, M (January 2005). "Incidence of autism spectrum disorders: changes over time and their meaning". Acta Paediatrica 94 (1): 2-15.
  47. Report to the Legislature on the Principal Findings of the Epidemiology of Autism in California Pilot Study. Retrieved on September 18, 2006.
  48. Stenson, Jacqueline. As autism cases soar, a search for clues, Newsweek, 24 February 2005.
  49. Posey DJ, Stigler KA, Erickson CA, McDougle CJ (2008). "Antipsychotics in the treatment of autism". J. Clin. Invest. 118 (1): 6–14. DOI:10.1172/JCI32483. PMID 18172517. Research Blogging.
  50. McCracken JT, McGough J, Shah B, Cronin P, Hong D, Aman MG et al. (2002). "Risperidone in children with autism and serious behavioral problems.". N Engl J Med 347 (5): 314-21. DOI:10.1056/NEJMoa013171. PMID 12151468. Research Blogging.
  51. "Study Provides Evidence That Autism Affects Functioning of Entire Brain-Previous View Held Autism Limited to Communication, Social Behavior, and Reasoning" National Institutes of Health, (Aug, 16, 2006)
  52. “Autism 'affects all of the brain'” BBC News (2006) Wednesday, 16 August
  53. “Clue to flaws in autistic brain” BBC News (2006) Saturday, 14 October
  54. 54.0 54.1 Pathophysiology of Autism: Brain Mechanisms M. Denckla (08/15/2006) National Institute of Mental Health
  55. Hardan, A., Minshew, N., Mallikarjuhn, M., Keshavan, M. (2001). Brain Volume in Autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 16, 421-424. [6]
  56. Autism and epilepsy: Cause, consequence, comorbidity, or coincidence?, Lidia Gabis, John Pomeroy, and Mary R. Andriola, in Epilepsy & Behavior 7 (2005) 652–656
  57. Oberman L., Hubbard E., McCleery J., Altschuler E., Ramachandran V., Pineda J. (2006). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders, Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190-198.
  58. Dapretto, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28-30.
  59. Penn Researchers Find Link Between Autism and Abnormal Blood-Vessel Function and Oxidative Stress. Retrieved on August 15, 2006.
  60. 60.0 60.1 Presence of Large Deletions in Kindreds with Autism Yu C., Dawson G., Munson J., D'Souza I., Osterling J., Estes A., Leutenegger A., Flodman P., Smith M., Raskind W., Spence M., McMahon W., Wijsman E., Schellenberg G. (2002). American Journal of Human Genetics, 71, 100-115.
  61. 61.0 61.1 61.2 DNA "glitches" tied to autism, researchers say Reuters Health (2007) March 15. Retrieved March 24, 2007
  62. The Autism Chromosome Rearrangement Database Retrieved November 19, 2006
  63. Bailey A., Le Couteur A., Gottesman I., Bolton P., Simonoff E., Yuzda E., Rutter M. (1995). Inheritability: Evidence of autism as a genetic disorder is supported by evidence from twin studies. In a British twin study. Psychological Medicine, 25, 63–77.
  64. Bolton P., MacDonald H., Pickles A., Rios P., Goode S., Crowson M., Bailey A., Rutter M. (1994). A case-control family history study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 877–900.
  65. When nerve cells can’t make contact Physorg.co, (2006) Sept. 22 Retrieved March 3, 2007
  66. 66.0 66.1 Gene Linked to Autism in Families with More Than One Affected Child National Institutes of Health News(2006) Oct. Retrieved March 3, 2007
  67. The neuronal scaffold protein Shank3 mediates signaling and biological function of the receptor tyrosine kinase Ret in epithelial cells G. Schuetz et al (2004) Journal of Cell Biology. Volume 167, Number 5, 945-952
  68. 68.0 68.1 Researchers Find Rare Mutations in SHANK3 Gene Are Associated with Autism Synopsis of information from Nature Genetics (January 2007). 4 citations. Cure Autism Now. Retrieved March 3, 2007
  69. Neuroligins Kristen Harris (2001) Cell adhesion at synapses Synapse Web, Laboratory of Synapse Structure and Function. Human Brain Project. National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of Drug Abuse
  70. Neuroligins Organize Excitatory and Inhibitory Postsynaptic Membrane E. R. Graf et al, (2004). Cell Issue 119, pages 1013-1026
  71. Neuroligin Mutations as a Cause of Autism K. Chew Autism Vox Retrieved March 3, 2007
  72. 72.0 72.1 Deletion 22q13 Syndrome M.C Phelan (2003) Orphanet.com
  73. December 2006; doi:10.1038/ng1933, Nature Genetics)
  74. Aschner M, Yao CP, Allen JW, Tan KH (2000). "Methylmercury alters glutamate transport in astrocytes". Neurochem. Int. 37 (2-3): 199–206. PMID 10812205[e]
  75. Palmer RF, Blanchard S, Stein Z, Mandell D, Miller C (2006). "Environmental mercury release, special education rates, and autism disorder: an ecological study of Texas". Health & place 12 (2): 203–9. DOI:10.1016/j.healthplace.2004.11.005. PMID 16338635. Research Blogging.